• Skip to main content
  • Skip to header right navigation
  • Skip to site footer
Fast, Accurate, Reliable Eye Tracking

高速、精准和可靠的眼动追踪解决方案

  • 硬件
    • EyeLink 1000 Plus
    • EyeLink Portable Duo
    • fMRI和MEG系统
    • EyeLink II
    • 硬件集成
  • 软件
    • Experiment Builder
    • Data Viewer
    • WebLink
    • 软件集成
    • Purchase Licenses
  • 解决方案
    • 阅读与语言
    • 发展研究
    • fMRI 和 MEG
    • EEG 和 fNIRS
    • 临床与眼动机制研究
    • 认知性
    • 可用性与应用研究
    • 非人类 灵长类动物
  • 技术支持
    • 论坛
    • 资源
    • 有用的应用程序
    • 训练
  • 关于
    • 关于我们
    • EyeLink出版物
    • 新闻
    • 制造
    • 职业生涯
    • 关于眼动追踪
    • 新闻通讯
  • 博客
  • 联系
  • English
eye tracking research

被高度引用的眼线文章

EyeLink Eye Tracker Publications

预计阅读时间: 3 分钟

我们最近完成了EyeLink出版物数据库的更新工作,仅在2019年就发表了900多篇论文,该数据库现在总共包含8000多篇出版物。每个出版物都要单独检查,以确保其中包含使用EyeLink眼动跟踪器收集的数据(而不仅仅是指使用EyeLink收集的数据,如元分析或评论文章中的数据),并确保该研究发表在同行评议的期刊上。

Publications by Year

在之前的博客中,我绘制了每年的出版物数量,该图的更新版本如下:

EyeLink Eye Tracking Publications 2019

被高度引用的眼线出版物

早些时候的博客还列出了眼线出版物的“顶级”期刊——无论是从眼线文章的数量还是从期刊的影响因素来看。今年,我想列出我们数据库中引用率最高的一些文章可能会很有趣。确定引文数量是一门有点不精确的科学。关于文章引用数量的信息主要有三个来源:科学网、Scopus和谷歌学者。虽然每种来源的优缺点都是一个激烈争论的话题(Harzing对此做了大量的文章–例如, 请参见本博客),但Google Scholar有两个优点,即覆盖面非常广泛,并且可以自由访问。

下面的列表是从15篇眼线文章中挑选出来的,根据谷歌学者的统计,所有这些文章的引用次数都超过500次。该列表是通过在我们的数据库中搜索按眼链接文章量排名前20位的期刊和按影响因子排名前10位的期刊而生成的。本文件并非详尽无遗,条款未按特定顺序列出。我认为,这份清单提供了一个迷人的例子,说明了眼线追踪者所参与的研究的广度(和巨大影响)。

Boer, Minke J.; Başkent, Deniz; Cornelissen, Frans W.

Eyes on emotion: Dynamic gaze allocation during emotion perception from speech-like stimuli Journal Article

In: Multisensory Research, vol. 34, no. 1, pp. 17–47, 2021.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Boer2021a,
title = {Eyes on emotion: Dynamic gaze allocation during emotion perception from speech-like stimuli},
author = {Minke J. Boer and Deniz Başkent and Frans W. Cornelissen},
doi = {10.1163/22134808-bja10029},
year = {2021},
date = {2021-01-01},
journal = {Multisensory Research},
volume = {34},
number = {1},
pages = {17--47},
abstract = {The majority of emotional expressions used in daily communication are multimodal and dynamic in nature. Consequently, one would expect that human observers utilize specific perceptual strategies to process emotions and to handle the multimodal and dynamic nature of emotions. However, our present knowledge on these strategies is scarce, primarily because most studies on emotion perception have not fully covered this variation, and instead used static and/or unimodal stimuli with few emotion categories. To resolve this knowledge gap, the present study examined how dynamic emotional auditory and visual information is integrated into a unified percept. Since there is a broad spectrum of possible forms of integration, both eye movements and accuracy of emotion identification were evaluated while observers performed an emotion identification task in one of three conditions: audio-only, visual-only video, or audiovisual video. In terms of adaptations of perceptual strategies, eye movement results showed a shift in fixations toward the eyes and away from the nose and mouth when audio is added. Notably, in terms of task performance, audio-only performance was mostly significantly worse than video-only and audiovisual performances, but performance in the latter two conditions was often not different. These results suggest that individuals flexibly and momentarily adapt their perceptual strategies to changes in the available information for emotion recognition, and these changes can be comprehensively quantified with eye tracking.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The majority of emotional expressions used in daily communication are multimodal and dynamic in nature. Consequently, one would expect that human observers utilize specific perceptual strategies to process emotions and to handle the multimodal and dynamic nature of emotions. However, our present knowledge on these strategies is scarce, primarily because most studies on emotion perception have not fully covered this variation, and instead used static and/or unimodal stimuli with few emotion categories. To resolve this knowledge gap, the present study examined how dynamic emotional auditory and visual information is integrated into a unified percept. Since there is a broad spectrum of possible forms of integration, both eye movements and accuracy of emotion identification were evaluated while observers performed an emotion identification task in one of three conditions: audio-only, visual-only video, or audiovisual video. In terms of adaptations of perceptual strategies, eye movement results showed a shift in fixations toward the eyes and away from the nose and mouth when audio is added. Notably, in terms of task performance, audio-only performance was mostly significantly worse than video-only and audiovisual performances, but performance in the latter two conditions was often not different. These results suggest that individuals flexibly and momentarily adapt their perceptual strategies to changes in the available information for emotion recognition, and these changes can be comprehensively quantified with eye tracking.

Close

  • doi:10.1163/22134808-bja10029

Close

McGarrigle, Ronan; Knight, Sarah; Rakusen, Lyndon; Geller, Jason; Mattys, Sven

Older adults show a more sustained pattern of effortful listening than young adults Journal Article

In: Psychology and Aging, vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 504–519, 2021.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{McGarrigle2021,
title = {Older adults show a more sustained pattern of effortful listening than young adults},
author = {Ronan McGarrigle and Sarah Knight and Lyndon Rakusen and Jason Geller and Sven Mattys},
doi = {10.1037/pag0000587},
year = {2021},
date = {2021-01-01},
journal = {Psychology and Aging},
volume = {36},
number = {4},
pages = {504--519},
abstract = {Listening to speech in adverse conditions can be challenging and effortful, especially for older adults. This study examined age-related differences in effortful listening by recording changes in the task-evoked pupil response (TEPR; a physiological marker of listening effort) both at the level of sentence processing and over the entire course of a listening task. A total of 65 (32 young adults, 33 older adults) participants performed a speech recognition task in the presence of a competing talker, while moment-to-moment changes in pupil size were continuously monitored. Participants were also administered the Vanderbilt Fatigue Scale, a questionnaire assessing daily life listening-related fatigue within four domains (social, cognitive, emotional, physical). Normalized TEPRs were overall larger and more steeply rising and falling around the peak in the older versus the young adult group during sentence processing. Additionally, mean TEPRs over the course of the listening task were more stable in the older versus the young adult group, consistent with a more sustained recruitment of compensatory attentional resources to maintain task performance. No age-related differences were found in terms of total daily life listening-related fatigue; however, older adults reported higher scores than young adults within the social domain. Overall, this study provides evidence for qualitatively distinct patterns of physiological arousal between young and older adults consistent with age-related upregulation in resource allocation during listening. A more detailed understanding of age-related changes in the subjective and physiological mechanisms that underlie effortful listening will ultimately help to address complex communication needs in aging listeners. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved)},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Listening to speech in adverse conditions can be challenging and effortful, especially for older adults. This study examined age-related differences in effortful listening by recording changes in the task-evoked pupil response (TEPR; a physiological marker of listening effort) both at the level of sentence processing and over the entire course of a listening task. A total of 65 (32 young adults, 33 older adults) participants performed a speech recognition task in the presence of a competing talker, while moment-to-moment changes in pupil size were continuously monitored. Participants were also administered the Vanderbilt Fatigue Scale, a questionnaire assessing daily life listening-related fatigue within four domains (social, cognitive, emotional, physical). Normalized TEPRs were overall larger and more steeply rising and falling around the peak in the older versus the young adult group during sentence processing. Additionally, mean TEPRs over the course of the listening task were more stable in the older versus the young adult group, consistent with a more sustained recruitment of compensatory attentional resources to maintain task performance. No age-related differences were found in terms of total daily life listening-related fatigue; however, older adults reported higher scores than young adults within the social domain. Overall, this study provides evidence for qualitatively distinct patterns of physiological arousal between young and older adults consistent with age-related upregulation in resource allocation during listening. A more detailed understanding of age-related changes in the subjective and physiological mechanisms that underlie effortful listening will ultimately help to address complex communication needs in aging listeners. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved)

Close

  • doi:10.1037/pag0000587

Close

Hintz, Florian; Meyer, Antje S.; Huettig, Falk

Visual context constrains language-mediated anticipatory eye movements Journal Article

In: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 73, no. 3, pp. 458–467, 2020.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Hintz2020,
title = {Visual context constrains language-mediated anticipatory eye movements},
author = {Florian Hintz and Antje S. Meyer and Falk Huettig},
doi = {10.1177/1747021819881615},
year = {2020},
date = {2020-01-01},
journal = {Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology},
volume = {73},
number = {3},
pages = {458--467},
abstract = {Contemporary accounts of anticipatory language processing assume that individuals predict upcoming information at multiple levels of representation. Research investigating language-mediated anticipatory eye gaze typically assumes that linguistic input restricts the domain of subsequent reference (visual target objects). Here, we explored the converse case: Can visual input restrict the dynamics of anticipatory language processing? To this end, we recorded participants' eye movements as they listened to sentences in which an object was predictable based on the verb's selectional restrictions (“The man peels a banana”). While listening, participants looked at different types of displays: the target object (banana) was either present or it was absent. On target-absent trials, the displays featured objects that had a similar visual shape as the target object (canoe) or objects that were semantically related to the concepts invoked by the target (monkey). Each trial was presented in a long preview version, where participants saw the displays for approximately 1.78 s before the verb was heard (pre-verb condition), and a short preview version, where participants saw the display approximately 1 s after the verb had been heard (post-verb condition), 750 ms prior to the spoken target onset. Participants anticipated the target objects in both conditions. Importantly, robust evidence for predictive looks to objects related to the (absent) target objects in visual shape and semantics was found in the post-verb but not in the pre-verb condition. These results suggest that visual information can restrict language-mediated anticipatory gaze and delineate theoretical accounts of predictive processing in the visual world.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Contemporary accounts of anticipatory language processing assume that individuals predict upcoming information at multiple levels of representation. Research investigating language-mediated anticipatory eye gaze typically assumes that linguistic input restricts the domain of subsequent reference (visual target objects). Here, we explored the converse case: Can visual input restrict the dynamics of anticipatory language processing? To this end, we recorded participants' eye movements as they listened to sentences in which an object was predictable based on the verb's selectional restrictions (“The man peels a banana”). While listening, participants looked at different types of displays: the target object (banana) was either present or it was absent. On target-absent trials, the displays featured objects that had a similar visual shape as the target object (canoe) or objects that were semantically related to the concepts invoked by the target (monkey). Each trial was presented in a long preview version, where participants saw the displays for approximately 1.78 s before the verb was heard (pre-verb condition), and a short preview version, where participants saw the display approximately 1 s after the verb had been heard (post-verb condition), 750 ms prior to the spoken target onset. Participants anticipated the target objects in both conditions. Importantly, robust evidence for predictive looks to objects related to the (absent) target objects in visual shape and semantics was found in the post-verb but not in the pre-verb condition. These results suggest that visual information can restrict language-mediated anticipatory gaze and delineate theoretical accounts of predictive processing in the visual world.

Close

  • doi:10.1177/1747021819881615

Close

Eger, Nikola Anna; Mitterer, Holger; Reinisch, Eva

Learning a new sound pair in a second language: Italian learners and German glottal consonants Journal Article

In: Journal of Phonetics, vol. 77, pp. 1–24, 2019.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Eger2019,
title = {Learning a new sound pair in a second language: Italian learners and German glottal consonants},
author = {Nikola Anna Eger and Holger Mitterer and Eva Reinisch},
doi = {10.1016/j.wocn.2019.100917},
year = {2019},
date = {2019-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Phonetics},
volume = {77},
pages = {1--24},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {The present study investigated Italian learners' production and perception of German /h/ and /Ɂ/ – two sounds that lack obvious linguistic counterparts in Italian. Critically, of these sounds only /h/ is explicitly known to learners from instruction and orthography. We therefore asked whether this awareness would lead to better acquisition of /h/ than /Ɂ/, and whether any differences would depend on the explicitness of the task. In production, learners of a medium proficiency level performed accurately in about 70% of the cases, with errors including sound deletions and substitutions. In spoken word recognition, two other learner groups of the same proficiency were hindered by sound deletions, but not by substitutions, although they were able to differentiate the sounds in an explicit goodness rating task. Overall, acquisition of /Ɂ/ was similar to /h/, despite lack of awareness for this sound. The results suggest that learners have established one combined “glottal category” to which both sounds map in speech processing, while they may be better implemented in production.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The present study investigated Italian learners' production and perception of German /h/ and /Ɂ/ – two sounds that lack obvious linguistic counterparts in Italian. Critically, of these sounds only /h/ is explicitly known to learners from instruction and orthography. We therefore asked whether this awareness would lead to better acquisition of /h/ than /Ɂ/, and whether any differences would depend on the explicitness of the task. In production, learners of a medium proficiency level performed accurately in about 70% of the cases, with errors including sound deletions and substitutions. In spoken word recognition, two other learner groups of the same proficiency were hindered by sound deletions, but not by substitutions, although they were able to differentiate the sounds in an explicit goodness rating task. Overall, acquisition of /Ɂ/ was similar to /h/, despite lack of awareness for this sound. The results suggest that learners have established one combined “glottal category” to which both sounds map in speech processing, while they may be better implemented in production.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.wocn.2019.100917

Close

Isabella, Silvia L.; Urbain, Charline; Cheyne, J. Allan; Cheyne, Douglas

Pupillary responses and reaction times index different cognitive processes in a combined Go/Switch incidental learning task Journal Article

In: Neuropsychologia, vol. 127, pp. 48–56, 2019.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Isabella2019,
title = {Pupillary responses and reaction times index different cognitive processes in a combined Go/Switch incidental learning task},
author = {Silvia L. Isabella and Charline Urbain and J. Allan Cheyne and Douglas Cheyne},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.02.007},
year = {2019},
date = {2019-01-01},
journal = {Neuropsychologia},
volume = {127},
pages = {48--56},
abstract = {In previous studies we have provided evidence that performance in speeded response tasks with infrequent target stimuli reflects both automatic and controlled cognitive processes, based on differences in reaction time (RT) and task-related brain responses (Cheyne et al. 2012, Isabella et al. 2015). Here we test the hypothesis that such shifts in cognitive control may be influenced by changes in cognitive load related to stimulus predictability, and that these changes can be indexed by task-evoked pupillary responses (TEPR). We manipulated stimulus predictability using fixed stimulus sequences that were unknown to the participants in a Go/Switch task (re-quiring a switch response on 25% of trials) while monitoring TEPR as a measure of cognitive load in 12 healthy adults. Results showed significant improvement in performance (reduced RT, increased efficiency) for repeated sequences compared to occasional deviant sequences (10% probability) indicating that incidental learning of the predictable sequences facilitated performance. All behavioral measures varied between Switch and Go trials (RT, efficiency), however mean TEPR amplitude (mTEPR) and latency to maximum pupil dilation were particularly sensitive to Go/Switch. Results were consistent with the hypothesis that mTEPR indexes cognitive load, whereas TEPR latency indexes time to response selection, independent from response execution. The present study provides evidence that incidental pattern learning during response inhibition tasks may modulate several cognitive processes including cognitive load, effort, response selection and execution, which can in turn have differential effects on measures of performance. In particular, we demonstrate that reaction time may not be indicative of underlying cognitive load.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

In previous studies we have provided evidence that performance in speeded response tasks with infrequent target stimuli reflects both automatic and controlled cognitive processes, based on differences in reaction time (RT) and task-related brain responses (Cheyne et al. 2012, Isabella et al. 2015). Here we test the hypothesis that such shifts in cognitive control may be influenced by changes in cognitive load related to stimulus predictability, and that these changes can be indexed by task-evoked pupillary responses (TEPR). We manipulated stimulus predictability using fixed stimulus sequences that were unknown to the participants in a Go/Switch task (re-quiring a switch response on 25% of trials) while monitoring TEPR as a measure of cognitive load in 12 healthy adults. Results showed significant improvement in performance (reduced RT, increased efficiency) for repeated sequences compared to occasional deviant sequences (10% probability) indicating that incidental learning of the predictable sequences facilitated performance. All behavioral measures varied between Switch and Go trials (RT, efficiency), however mean TEPR amplitude (mTEPR) and latency to maximum pupil dilation were particularly sensitive to Go/Switch. Results were consistent with the hypothesis that mTEPR indexes cognitive load, whereas TEPR latency indexes time to response selection, independent from response execution. The present study provides evidence that incidental pattern learning during response inhibition tasks may modulate several cognitive processes including cognitive load, effort, response selection and execution, which can in turn have differential effects on measures of performance. In particular, we demonstrate that reaction time may not be indicative of underlying cognitive load.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.02.007

Close

Seemiller, Eric S.; Port, Nicholas L.; Candy, T. Rowan

The gaze stability of 4- to 10-week-old human infants Journal Article

In: Journal of Vision, vol. 18, no. 8, pp. 1–10, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Seemiller2018,
title = {The gaze stability of 4- to 10-week-old human infants},
author = {Eric S. Seemiller and Nicholas L. Port and T. Rowan Candy},
doi = {10.1167/18.8.15},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Vision},
volume = {18},
number = {8},
pages = {1--10},
abstract = {The relationship between gaze stability, retinal image quality, and visual perception is complex. Gaze instability related to pathology in adults can cause a reduction in visual acuity (e.g., Chung, LaFrance, & Bedell, 2011). Conversely, poor retinal image quality and spatial vision may be a contributing factor to gaze instability (e.g., Ukwade & Bedell, 1993). Though much is known about the immaturities in spatial vision of human infants, little is currently understood about their gaze stability. To characterize the gaze stability of young infants, adult participants and 4- to 10-week-old infants were shown a dynamic random-noise stimulus for 30-s intervals while their eye positions were recorded binocularly. After removing adultlike saccades, we used 5-s epochs of stable intersaccade gaze to estimate bivariate contour ellipse area and standard deviations of vergence. The geometric means (with standard deviations) for infants' bivariate contour ellipse area were left eye = -0.697 ± 0.534 log(°2), right eye = -0.471 ± 0.367 log(°2). For binocular vergence stability, the infant geometric means (with standard deviations) were horizontal = -1.057 ± 0.743 log(°)},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The relationship between gaze stability, retinal image quality, and visual perception is complex. Gaze instability related to pathology in adults can cause a reduction in visual acuity (e.g., Chung, LaFrance, & Bedell, 2011). Conversely, poor retinal image quality and spatial vision may be a contributing factor to gaze instability (e.g., Ukwade & Bedell, 1993). Though much is known about the immaturities in spatial vision of human infants, little is currently understood about their gaze stability. To characterize the gaze stability of young infants, adult participants and 4- to 10-week-old infants were shown a dynamic random-noise stimulus for 30-s intervals while their eye positions were recorded binocularly. After removing adultlike saccades, we used 5-s epochs of stable intersaccade gaze to estimate bivariate contour ellipse area and standard deviations of vergence. The geometric means (with standard deviations) for infants' bivariate contour ellipse area were left eye = -0.697 ± 0.534 log(°2), right eye = -0.471 ± 0.367 log(°2). For binocular vergence stability, the infant geometric means (with standard deviations) were horizontal = -1.057 ± 0.743 log(°)

Close

  • doi:10.1167/18.8.15

Close

Hayes, Taylor R.; Petrov, Alexander A.

Mapping and correcting the influence of gaze position on pupil size measurements Journal Article

In: Behavior Research Methods, vol. 48, no. 2, pp. 510–527, 2016.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Hayes2016,
title = {Mapping and correcting the influence of gaze position on pupil size measurements},
author = {Taylor R. Hayes and Alexander A. Petrov},
doi = {10.3758/s13428-015-0588-x},
year = {2016},
date = {2016-01-01},
journal = {Behavior Research Methods},
volume = {48},
number = {2},
pages = {510--527},
abstract = {Pupil size is correlated with a wide variety of important cognitive variables and is increasingly being used by cognitive scientists. Pupil data can be recorded inexpensively and non-invasively by many commonly used video-based eye-tracking cameras. Despite the relative ease of data collection and increasing prevalence of pupil data in the cognitive literature, researchers often underestimate the methodological challenges associated with controlling for confounds that can result in misinterpretation of their data. One serious confound that is often not properly controlled is pupil foreshortening error (PFE)-the foreshortening of the pupil image as the eye rotates away from the camera. Here we systematically map PFE using an artificial eye model and then apply a geometric model correction. Three artificial eyes with different fixed pupil sizes were used to systematically measure changes in pupil size as a function of gaze position with a desktop EyeLink 1000 tracker. A grid-based map of pupil measurements was recorded with each artificial eye across three experimental layouts of the eye-tracking camera and display. Large, systematic deviations in pupil size were observed across all nine maps. The measured PFE was corrected by a geometric model that expressed the foreshortening of the pupil area as a function of the cosine of the angle between the eye-to-camera axis and the eye-to-stimulus axis. The model reduced the root mean squared error of pupil measurements by 82.5 % when the model parameters were pre-set to the physical layout dimensions, and by 97.5 % when they were optimized to fit the empirical error surface.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Pupil size is correlated with a wide variety of important cognitive variables and is increasingly being used by cognitive scientists. Pupil data can be recorded inexpensively and non-invasively by many commonly used video-based eye-tracking cameras. Despite the relative ease of data collection and increasing prevalence of pupil data in the cognitive literature, researchers often underestimate the methodological challenges associated with controlling for confounds that can result in misinterpretation of their data. One serious confound that is often not properly controlled is pupil foreshortening error (PFE)-the foreshortening of the pupil image as the eye rotates away from the camera. Here we systematically map PFE using an artificial eye model and then apply a geometric model correction. Three artificial eyes with different fixed pupil sizes were used to systematically measure changes in pupil size as a function of gaze position with a desktop EyeLink 1000 tracker. A grid-based map of pupil measurements was recorded with each artificial eye across three experimental layouts of the eye-tracking camera and display. Large, systematic deviations in pupil size were observed across all nine maps. The measured PFE was corrected by a geometric model that expressed the foreshortening of the pupil area as a function of the cosine of the angle between the eye-to-camera axis and the eye-to-stimulus axis. The model reduced the root mean squared error of pupil measurements by 82.5 % when the model parameters were pre-set to the physical layout dimensions, and by 97.5 % when they were optimized to fit the empirical error surface.

Close

  • doi:10.3758/s13428-015-0588-x

Close

Farmer, Thomas A.; Yan, Shaorong; Bicknell, Klinton; Tanenhaus, Michael K.

Form-to-expectation matching effects on first-pass eye movement measures during reading Journal Article

In: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol. 41, no. 4, pp. 958–976, 2015.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Farmer2015,
title = {Form-to-expectation matching effects on first-pass eye movement measures during reading},
author = {Thomas A. Farmer and Shaorong Yan and Klinton Bicknell and Michael K. Tanenhaus},
doi = {10.1037/xhp0000054},
year = {2015},
date = {2015-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance},
volume = {41},
number = {4},
pages = {958--976},
publisher = {Levy},
abstract = {Recent Electroencephalography/Magnetoencephalography (EEG/MEG) studies suggest that when contextual information is highly predictive of some property of a linguistic signal, expectations generated from context can be translated into surprisingly low-level estimates of the physical form-based properties likely to occur in subsequent portions of the unfolding signal. Whether form-based expectations are generated and assessed during natural reading, however, remains unclear. We monitored eye movements while participants read phonologically typical and atypical nouns in noun-predictive contexts (Experiment 1), demonstrating that when a noun is strongly expected, fixation durations on furst-pass eye movement measures, including first fixation duration, gaze durations, and go-past times, are shorter for nouns with category typical form-based features. In Experiments 2 and 3, typical and atypical nouns were placed in sentential contexts normed to create expectations of variable strength for a noun. Context and typicality interacted significantly at gaze duration. These results suggest that during reading, form-based expectations that are translated from higher-level category-based expectancies can facilitate the processing of a word in context, and that their effect on lexical processing is graded based on the strength of category expectancy.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Recent Electroencephalography/Magnetoencephalography (EEG/MEG) studies suggest that when contextual information is highly predictive of some property of a linguistic signal, expectations generated from context can be translated into surprisingly low-level estimates of the physical form-based properties likely to occur in subsequent portions of the unfolding signal. Whether form-based expectations are generated and assessed during natural reading, however, remains unclear. We monitored eye movements while participants read phonologically typical and atypical nouns in noun-predictive contexts (Experiment 1), demonstrating that when a noun is strongly expected, fixation durations on furst-pass eye movement measures, including first fixation duration, gaze durations, and go-past times, are shorter for nouns with category typical form-based features. In Experiments 2 and 3, typical and atypical nouns were placed in sentential contexts normed to create expectations of variable strength for a noun. Context and typicality interacted significantly at gaze duration. These results suggest that during reading, form-based expectations that are translated from higher-level category-based expectancies can facilitate the processing of a word in context, and that their effect on lexical processing is graded based on the strength of category expectancy.

Close

  • doi:10.1037/xhp0000054

Close

Liu, Yanping; Reichle, Erik D.; Li, Xingshan

Parafoveal processing affects outgoing saccade length during the reading of Chinese Journal Article

In: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, vol. 41, no. 4, pp. 1229–1236, 2015.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Liu2015b,
title = {Parafoveal processing affects outgoing saccade length during the reading of Chinese},
author = {Yanping Liu and Erik D. Reichle and Xingshan Li},
doi = {10.1037/xlm0000057},
year = {2015},
date = {2015-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition},
volume = {41},
number = {4},
pages = {1229--1236},
abstract = {Participants' eye movements were measured while reading Chinese sentences in which target-word frequency and the availability of parafoveal processing were manipulated using a gaze-contingent boundary paradigm. The results of this study indicate that preview availability and its interaction with word frequency modulated the length of the saccades exiting the target words, suggesting important functional roles for parafoveal processing in determining where the eyes move during reading. The theoretical significance of these findings is discussed in relation to 2 current models of eye-movement control during reading, both of which assume that saccades are directed toward default targets (e.g., the center of the next unidentified word). A possible method for addressing these limitations (i.e., dynamic attention allocation) is also discussed.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Participants' eye movements were measured while reading Chinese sentences in which target-word frequency and the availability of parafoveal processing were manipulated using a gaze-contingent boundary paradigm. The results of this study indicate that preview availability and its interaction with word frequency modulated the length of the saccades exiting the target words, suggesting important functional roles for parafoveal processing in determining where the eyes move during reading. The theoretical significance of these findings is discussed in relation to 2 current models of eye-movement control during reading, both of which assume that saccades are directed toward default targets (e.g., the center of the next unidentified word). A possible method for addressing these limitations (i.e., dynamic attention allocation) is also discussed.

Close

  • doi:10.1037/xlm0000057

Close

Perez-Osorio, Jairo; Müller, Hermann J.; Wiese, Eva; Wykowska, Agnieszka

Gaze following is modulated by expectations regarding others' action goals Journal Article

In: PLoS ONE, vol. 10, no. 11, pp. e0143614, 2015.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{PerezOsorio2015,
title = {Gaze following is modulated by expectations regarding others' action goals},
author = {Jairo Perez-Osorio and Hermann J. Müller and Eva Wiese and Agnieszka Wykowska},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0143614},
year = {2015},
date = {2015-01-01},
journal = {PLoS ONE},
volume = {10},
number = {11},
pages = {e0143614},
abstract = {Humans attend to social cues in order to understand and predict others' behavior. Facial expressions and gaze direction provide valuable information to infer others' mental states and intentions. The present study examined the mechanism of gaze following in the context of participants' expectations about successive action steps of an observed actor. We embedded a gaze-cueing manipulation within an action scenario consisting of a sequence of naturalistic photographs. Gaze-induced orienting of attention (gaze following) was analyzed with respect to whether the gaze behavior of the observed actor was in line or not with the action-related expectations of participants (i.e., whether the actor gazed at an object that was congruent or incongruent with an overarching action goal). In Experiment 1, participants followed the gaze of the observed agent, though the gaze-cueing effect was larger when the actor looked at an action-congruent object relative to an incongruent object. Experiment 2 examined whether the pattern of effects observed in Experiment 1 was due to covert, rather than overt, attentional orienting, by requiring participants to maintain eye fixation throughout the sequence of critical photographs (corroborated bymonitoring eye movements). The essential pattern of results of Experiment 1 was replicated, with the gaze- cueing effect being completely eliminated when the observed agent gazed at an action-incongruent object. Thus, our findings show that covert gaze following can be modulated by expectations that humans hold regarding successive steps of the action performed by an observed agent.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Humans attend to social cues in order to understand and predict others' behavior. Facial expressions and gaze direction provide valuable information to infer others' mental states and intentions. The present study examined the mechanism of gaze following in the context of participants' expectations about successive action steps of an observed actor. We embedded a gaze-cueing manipulation within an action scenario consisting of a sequence of naturalistic photographs. Gaze-induced orienting of attention (gaze following) was analyzed with respect to whether the gaze behavior of the observed actor was in line or not with the action-related expectations of participants (i.e., whether the actor gazed at an object that was congruent or incongruent with an overarching action goal). In Experiment 1, participants followed the gaze of the observed agent, though the gaze-cueing effect was larger when the actor looked at an action-congruent object relative to an incongruent object. Experiment 2 examined whether the pattern of effects observed in Experiment 1 was due to covert, rather than overt, attentional orienting, by requiring participants to maintain eye fixation throughout the sequence of critical photographs (corroborated bymonitoring eye movements). The essential pattern of results of Experiment 1 was replicated, with the gaze- cueing effect being completely eliminated when the observed agent gazed at an action-incongruent object. Thus, our findings show that covert gaze following can be modulated by expectations that humans hold regarding successive steps of the action performed by an observed agent.

Close

  • doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0143614

Close

Shelton, Annie L.; Cornish, Kim M.; Kraan, Claudine; Georgiou-Karistianis, Nellie; Metcalfe, Sylvia A.; Bradshaw, John L.; Hocking, Darren R.; Archibald, Alison D.; Cohen, Jonathan; Trollor, Julian N.; Fielding, Joanne

Exploring inhibitory deficits in female premutation carriers of fragile X syndrome: Through eye movements Journal Article

In: Brain and Cognition, vol. 85, no. 1, pp. 201–208, 2014.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Shelton2014,
title = {Exploring inhibitory deficits in female premutation carriers of fragile X syndrome: Through eye movements},
author = {Annie L. Shelton and Kim M. Cornish and Claudine Kraan and Nellie Georgiou-Karistianis and Sylvia A. Metcalfe and John L. Bradshaw and Darren R. Hocking and Alison D. Archibald and Jonathan Cohen and Julian N. Trollor and Joanne Fielding},
doi = {10.1016/j.bandc.2013.12.006},
year = {2014},
date = {2014-01-01},
journal = {Brain and Cognition},
volume = {85},
number = {1},
pages = {201--208},
publisher = {Elsevier Inc.},
abstract = {There is evidence which demonstrates that a subset of males with a premutation CGG repeat expansion (between 55 and 200 repeats) of the fragile X mental retardation 1 gene exhibit subtle deficits of executive function that progressively deteriorate with increasing age and CGG repeat length. However, it remains unclear whether similar deficits, which may indicate the onset of more severe degeneration, are evident in female PM-carriers. In the present study we explore whether female PM-carriers exhibit deficits of executive function which parallel those of male PM-carriers. Fourteen female fragile X premutation carriers without fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome and fourteen age, sex, and IQ matched controls underwent ocular motor and neuropsychological tests of select executive processes, specifically of response inhibition and working memory. Group comparisons revealed poorer inhibitory control for female premutation carriers on ocular motor tasks, in addition to demonstrating some difficulties in behaviour self-regulation, when compared to controls. A negative correlation between CGG repeat length and antisaccade error rates for premutation carriers was also found. Our preliminary findings indicate that impaired inhibitory control may represent a phenotype characteristic which may be a sensitive risk biomarker within this female fragile X premutation population.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

There is evidence which demonstrates that a subset of males with a premutation CGG repeat expansion (between 55 and 200 repeats) of the fragile X mental retardation 1 gene exhibit subtle deficits of executive function that progressively deteriorate with increasing age and CGG repeat length. However, it remains unclear whether similar deficits, which may indicate the onset of more severe degeneration, are evident in female PM-carriers. In the present study we explore whether female PM-carriers exhibit deficits of executive function which parallel those of male PM-carriers. Fourteen female fragile X premutation carriers without fragile X-associated tremor/ataxia syndrome and fourteen age, sex, and IQ matched controls underwent ocular motor and neuropsychological tests of select executive processes, specifically of response inhibition and working memory. Group comparisons revealed poorer inhibitory control for female premutation carriers on ocular motor tasks, in addition to demonstrating some difficulties in behaviour self-regulation, when compared to controls. A negative correlation between CGG repeat length and antisaccade error rates for premutation carriers was also found. Our preliminary findings indicate that impaired inhibitory control may represent a phenotype characteristic which may be a sensitive risk biomarker within this female fragile X premutation population.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2013.12.006

Close

Bate, Sarah; Haslam, Catherine; Hodgson, Timothy L.; Jansari, Ashok; Gregory, Nicola J.; Kay, Janice

Positive and negative emotion enhances the processing of famous faces in a semantic judgment task Journal Article

In: Neuropsychology, vol. 24, no. 1, pp. 84–89, 2010.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Bate2010,
title = {Positive and negative emotion enhances the processing of famous faces in a semantic judgment task},
author = {Sarah Bate and Catherine Haslam and Timothy L. Hodgson and Ashok Jansari and Nicola J. Gregory and Janice Kay},
doi = {10.1037/a0017202},
year = {2010},
date = {2010-01-01},
journal = {Neuropsychology},
volume = {24},
number = {1},
pages = {84--89},
abstract = {Previous work has consistently reported a facilitatory influence of positive emotion in face recognition (e.g., D'Argembeau, Van der Linden, Comblain, & Etienne, 2003). However, these reports asked participants to make recognition judgments in response to faces, and it is unknown whether emotional valence may influence other stages of processing, such as at the level of semantics. Furthermore, other evidence suggests that negative rather than positive emotion facilitates higher level judgments when processing nonfacial stimuli (e.g., Mickley & Kensinger, 2008), and it is possible that negative emotion also influences latter stages of face processing. The present study addressed this issue, examining the influence of emotional valence while participants made semantic judgments in response to a set of famous faces. Eye movements were monitored while participants performed this task, and analyses revealed a reduction in information extraction for the faces of liked and disliked celebrities compared with those of emotionally neutral celebrities. Thus, in contrast to work using familiarity judgments, both positive and negative emotion facilitated processing in this semantic-based task. This pattern of findings is discussed in relation to current models of face processing.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Previous work has consistently reported a facilitatory influence of positive emotion in face recognition (e.g., D'Argembeau, Van der Linden, Comblain, & Etienne, 2003). However, these reports asked participants to make recognition judgments in response to faces, and it is unknown whether emotional valence may influence other stages of processing, such as at the level of semantics. Furthermore, other evidence suggests that negative rather than positive emotion facilitates higher level judgments when processing nonfacial stimuli (e.g., Mickley & Kensinger, 2008), and it is possible that negative emotion also influences latter stages of face processing. The present study addressed this issue, examining the influence of emotional valence while participants made semantic judgments in response to a set of famous faces. Eye movements were monitored while participants performed this task, and analyses revealed a reduction in information extraction for the faces of liked and disliked celebrities compared with those of emotionally neutral celebrities. Thus, in contrast to work using familiarity judgments, both positive and negative emotion facilitated processing in this semantic-based task. This pattern of findings is discussed in relation to current models of face processing.

Close

  • doi:10.1037/a0017202

Close

Nummenmaa, Lauri; Hirvonen, Jussi; Parkkola, Riitta; Hietanen, Jari K.

Is emotional contagion special? An fMRI study on neural systems for affective and cognitive empathy Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 43, no. 3, pp. 571–580, 2008.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Nummenmaa2008,
title = {Is emotional contagion special? An fMRI study on neural systems for affective and cognitive empathy},
author = {Lauri Nummenmaa and Jussi Hirvonen and Riitta Parkkola and Jari K. Hietanen},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.014},
year = {2008},
date = {2008-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {43},
number = {3},
pages = {571--580},
publisher = {Elsevier Inc.},
abstract = {Empathy allows us to simulate others' affective and cognitive mental states internally, and it has been proposed that the mirroring or motor representation systems play a key role in such simulation. As emotions are related to important adaptive events linked with benefit or danger, simulating others' emotional states might constitute of a special case of empathy. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study we tested if emotional versus cognitive empathy would facilitate the recruitment of brain networks involved in motor representation and imitation in healthy volunteers. Participants were presented with photographs depicting people in neutral everyday situations (cognitive empathy blocks), or suffering serious threat or harm (emotional empathy blocks). Participants were instructed to empathize with specified persons depicted in the scenes. Emotional versus cognitive empathy resulted in increased activity in limbic areas involved in emotion processing (thalamus), and also in cortical areas involved in face (fusiform gyrus) and body perception, as well as in networks associated with mirroring of others' actions (inferior parietal lobule). When brain activation resulting from viewing the scenes was controlled, emotional empathy still engaged the mirror neuron system (premotor cortex) more than cognitive empathy. Further, thalamus and primary somatosensory and motor cortices showed increased functional coupling during emotional versus cognitive empathy. The results suggest that emotional empathy is special. Emotional empathy facilitates somatic, sensory, and motor representation of other peoples' mental states, and results in more vigorous mirroring of the observed mental and bodily states than cognitive empathy.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Empathy allows us to simulate others' affective and cognitive mental states internally, and it has been proposed that the mirroring or motor representation systems play a key role in such simulation. As emotions are related to important adaptive events linked with benefit or danger, simulating others' emotional states might constitute of a special case of empathy. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study we tested if emotional versus cognitive empathy would facilitate the recruitment of brain networks involved in motor representation and imitation in healthy volunteers. Participants were presented with photographs depicting people in neutral everyday situations (cognitive empathy blocks), or suffering serious threat or harm (emotional empathy blocks). Participants were instructed to empathize with specified persons depicted in the scenes. Emotional versus cognitive empathy resulted in increased activity in limbic areas involved in emotion processing (thalamus), and also in cortical areas involved in face (fusiform gyrus) and body perception, as well as in networks associated with mirroring of others' actions (inferior parietal lobule). When brain activation resulting from viewing the scenes was controlled, emotional empathy still engaged the mirror neuron system (premotor cortex) more than cognitive empathy. Further, thalamus and primary somatosensory and motor cortices showed increased functional coupling during emotional versus cognitive empathy. The results suggest that emotional empathy is special. Emotional empathy facilitates somatic, sensory, and motor representation of other peoples' mental states, and results in more vigorous mirroring of the observed mental and bodily states than cognitive empathy.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.014

Close

Ryan, Jennifer D.; Shen, Jiye; Reingold, Eyal M.

Modulation of distraction in ageing Journal Article

In: British Journal of Psychology, vol. 97, no. 3, pp. 339–351, 2006.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Ryan2006,
title = {Modulation of distraction in ageing},
author = {Jennifer D. Ryan and Jiye Shen and Eyal M. Reingold},
doi = {10.1348/000712605X74837},
year = {2006},
date = {2006-01-01},
journal = {British Journal of Psychology},
volume = {97},
number = {3},
pages = {339--351},
abstract = {A cueing paradigm was employed to examine modulation of distraction due to a visual singleton. Subjects were required to make a saccade to a shape-singleton target. A predictive location cue indicated the hemifield where a target would appear. Older adults made more anticipatory saccades than younger adults, and were less accurate for making an eye movement in the vicinity of a target. However, younger and older adults likewise benefited from the cue; distraction was reduced when the distractor singleton appeared in an uncued hemisphere. The ability to compensate for problems with distraction in older and younger adults through use of the precue suggests that attention to a general region of space, rather than a specific location, may be enough to modulate distraction.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

A cueing paradigm was employed to examine modulation of distraction due to a visual singleton. Subjects were required to make a saccade to a shape-singleton target. A predictive location cue indicated the hemifield where a target would appear. Older adults made more anticipatory saccades than younger adults, and were less accurate for making an eye movement in the vicinity of a target. However, younger and older adults likewise benefited from the cue; distraction was reduced when the distractor singleton appeared in an uncued hemisphere. The ability to compensate for problems with distraction in older and younger adults through use of the precue suggests that attention to a general region of space, rather than a specific location, may be enough to modulate distraction.

Close

  • doi:10.1348/000712605X74837

Close

Bertram, Raymond; Hyönä, Jukka

The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds Journal Article

In: Journal of Memory and Language, vol. 48, pp. 615–634, 2003.

Abstract | BibTeX

@article{Bertram2003,
title = {The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds},
author = {Raymond Bertram and Jukka Hyönä},
year = {2003},
date = {2003-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Memory and Language},
volume = {48},
pages = {615--634},
abstract = {This study explored whether the length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure in lexical processing: Does morphological structure play a similar role in short complex words that typically elicit one eye fixation (e.g., eyelid) as it does in long complex words that typically elicit two or more eye fixations (e.g., watercourse)? Two eye movement experiments with short vs. long Finnish compound words in context were conducted to find an answer to this question. In Experiment 1, a first-constituent frequency manipulation revealed solid effects for long compounds in early and late processing measures, but no effects for short compounds. In contrast, in Experiment 2, a whole-word frequency manipulation elicited solid effects for short compounds in early and late processing measures, but mainly late effects for long compounds. A race model, incorporating a headstart for the decomposition route, in case whole-word information of complex words cannot be extracted in a single fixation can explain the pattern of results.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

This study explored whether the length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure in lexical processing: Does morphological structure play a similar role in short complex words that typically elicit one eye fixation (e.g., eyelid) as it does in long complex words that typically elicit two or more eye fixations (e.g., watercourse)? Two eye movement experiments with short vs. long Finnish compound words in context were conducted to find an answer to this question. In Experiment 1, a first-constituent frequency manipulation revealed solid effects for long compounds in early and late processing measures, but no effects for short compounds. In contrast, in Experiment 2, a whole-word frequency manipulation elicited solid effects for short compounds in early and late processing measures, but mainly late effects for long compounds. A race model, incorporating a headstart for the decomposition route, in case whole-word information of complex words cannot be extracted in a single fixation can explain the pattern of results.

Close

联系

如果您希望我们为您的眼线研究提供特色,对帖子有想法,或者对我们的硬件和软件有任何疑问,请与我们联系。我们总是很乐意帮忙。您可以致电我们(+1-613-271-8686)或单击下面的按钮发送电子邮件:

联系

图像信用

  1. Hermann的标题图像。

阅读更多信息

Previous Post:Evidence for Implicit Theory of Mind内隐心理理论的证据
Next Post:婴儿什么时候追踪物体?when do babies track an object

关于 Sam Hutton

Sam Hutton在Sussex的University学习实验心理学,他非常喜欢实验心理学,于是留在那里攻读博士学位。他第一次接触到眼动跟踪器是在他在London Imperial College的博士后研究期间。他在查林十字医院的神经眼科工作了6年,在那里,他学会了使用一种古老的红外眼球跟踪系统来测量神经精神病和神经疾病患者的基本动眼神经功能(prosaccades/antissaccades/smooth pursuit等)。他上瘾了,从那以后,他一直以各种方式积极参与眼球追踪研究。在不写博客时(例如,大部分时间…),他与SR Research支持团队的其他成员一起参与一系列项目,特别关注临床环境中的眼球跟踪问题(例如眼球跟踪眼震/神经系统疾病)和瞳孔测量。他还可以在一系列主题的眼动追踪研讨会上授课,并且通常试图确保人们不会犯与他相同的错误。他喜欢观察动物,到目前为止,他已经成功地记录了老鼠、猫、狗和鸭子的凝视…

让我们保持联系

  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • Instagram
  • LinkedIn
  • YouTube
新闻通讯
新闻通讯存档
会议

联系

info@sr-research.com

电话: +1-613-271-8686

免费电话: +1-866-821-0731

传真: +1-613-482-4866

快捷链接

产品

解决方案

技术支持

法律信息

法律声明

隐私政策 | 可访性政策

EyeLink® 眼动仪是研究设备,不能用于医疗诊断或治疗。

特色博客

Reading Profiles of Adults with Dyslexia

成人阅读障碍的阅读概况


Copyright © 2023 · SR Research Ltd. All Rights Reserved. EyeLink is a registered trademark of SR Research Ltd.