• Skip to main content
  • Skip to header right navigation
  • Skip to site footer
Fast, Accurate, Reliable Eye Tracking

Fast, Accurate, Reliable Eye Tracking

  • Hardware
    • EyeLink 1000 Plus
    • EyeLink Portable Duo
    • fMRI and MEG Systems
    • EyeLink II
    • Hardware Integration
  • Software
    • Experiment Builder
    • Data Viewer
    • WebLink
    • Software Integration
    • Purchase Licenses
  • Solutions
    • Reading and Language
    • Developmental
    • fMRI and MEG
    • EEG and fNIRS
    • Clinical and Oculomotor
    • Cognitive
    • Usability and Applied
    • Non Human Primate
  • Support
    • Forum
    • Resources
    • Useful Apps
    • Training
  • About
    • About Us
    • EyeLink Publications
    • Events
    • Manufacturing
    • Careers
    • About Eye Tracking
    • Newsletter
  • Blog
  • Contact
  • 简体中文
eye tracking research

fMRI / MEG Publications

EyeLink fMRI / MEG Publications

All EyeLink fMRI and MEG research publications (with concurrent eye tracking) up until 2022 (with some early 2023s) are listed below by year. You can search the publications using keywords such as Visual Cortex, Neural Plasticity, MEG, etc. You can also search for individual author names. If we missed any EyeLink fMRI or MEG articles, please email us!

575 entries « ‹ 4 of 6 › »

2018

Johannes Bloechle; Stefan Huber; Elise Klein; Julia Bahnmueller; Johannes Rennig; Korbinian Moeller; Julia F. Huber

Spatial arrangement and set size influence the coding of non-symbolic quantities in the intraparietal sulcus Journal Article

In: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 12, pp. 54, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Bloechle2018a,
title = {Spatial arrangement and set size influence the coding of non-symbolic quantities in the intraparietal sulcus},
author = {Johannes Bloechle and Stefan Huber and Elise Klein and Julia Bahnmueller and Johannes Rennig and Korbinian Moeller and Julia F. Huber},
doi = {10.3389/fnhum.2018.00054},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Frontiers in Human Neuroscience},
volume = {12},
pages = {54},
abstract = {Performance in visual quantification tasks shows two characteristic patterns as a function of set size. A precise subitizing process for small sets (up to four) was contrasted with an approximate estimation process for larger sets. The spatial arrangement of elements in a set also influences visual quantification performance, with frequently perceived arrangements (e.g., dice patterns) being faster enumerated than random arrangements. Neuropsychological and imaging studies identified the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), as key brain area for quantification, both within and above the subitizing range. However, it is not yet clear if and how set size and spatial arrangement of elements in a set modulate IPS activity during quantification. In an fMRI study, participants enumerated briefly presented dot patterns with random, canonical or dice arrangement within and above the subitizing range. We evaluated how activity amplitude and pattern in the IPS were influenced by size and spatial arrangement of a set. We found a discontinuity in the amplitude of IPS response between subitizing and estimation range, with steep activity increase for sets exceeding four elements. In the estimation range, random dot arrangements elicited stronger IPS response than canonical arrangements which in turn elicited stronger response than dice arrangements. Furthermore, IPS activity patterns differed systematically between arrangements. We found a signature in the IPS response for a transition between subitizing and estimation processes during quantification. Differences in amplitude and pattern of IPS activity for different spatial arrangements indicated a more precise representation of non-symbolic numerical magnitude for dice and canonical than for random arrangements. These findings challenge the idea of an abstract coding of numerosity in the IPS even within a single notation.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Performance in visual quantification tasks shows two characteristic patterns as a function of set size. A precise subitizing process for small sets (up to four) was contrasted with an approximate estimation process for larger sets. The spatial arrangement of elements in a set also influences visual quantification performance, with frequently perceived arrangements (e.g., dice patterns) being faster enumerated than random arrangements. Neuropsychological and imaging studies identified the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), as key brain area for quantification, both within and above the subitizing range. However, it is not yet clear if and how set size and spatial arrangement of elements in a set modulate IPS activity during quantification. In an fMRI study, participants enumerated briefly presented dot patterns with random, canonical or dice arrangement within and above the subitizing range. We evaluated how activity amplitude and pattern in the IPS were influenced by size and spatial arrangement of a set. We found a discontinuity in the amplitude of IPS response between subitizing and estimation range, with steep activity increase for sets exceeding four elements. In the estimation range, random dot arrangements elicited stronger IPS response than canonical arrangements which in turn elicited stronger response than dice arrangements. Furthermore, IPS activity patterns differed systematically between arrangements. We found a signature in the IPS response for a transition between subitizing and estimation processes during quantification. Differences in amplitude and pattern of IPS activity for different spatial arrangements indicated a more precise representation of non-symbolic numerical magnitude for dice and canonical than for random arrangements. These findings challenge the idea of an abstract coding of numerosity in the IPS even within a single notation.

Close

  • doi:10.3389/fnhum.2018.00054

Close

Michael B. Bone; Marie St-Laurent; Christa Dang; Douglas A. McQuiggan; Jennifer D. Ryan; Bradley R. Buchsbaum; Jennifer D. Ryan; Christa Dang; Michael B. Bone; Marie St-Laurent

Eye movement reinstatement and neural reactivation during mental imagery Journal Article

In: Cerebral Cortex, vol. 29, no. 3, pp. 1075–1089, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Bone2018,
title = {Eye movement reinstatement and neural reactivation during mental imagery},
author = {Michael B. Bone and Marie St-Laurent and Christa Dang and Douglas A. McQuiggan and Jennifer D. Ryan and Bradley R. Buchsbaum and Jennifer D. Ryan and Christa Dang and Michael B. Bone and Marie St-Laurent},
doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhy014},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
volume = {29},
number = {3},
pages = {1075--1089},
abstract = {Half a century ago, Donald Hebb posited that mental imagery is a constructive process that emulates perception. Specifically, Hebb claimed that visual imagery results from the reactivation of neural activity associated with viewing images. He also argued that neural reactivation and imagery benefit from the re-enactment of eye movement patterns that first occurred at viewing (fixation reinstatement). To investigate these claims, we applied multivariate pattern analyses to functional MRI (fMRI) and eye-tracking data collected while healthy human participants repeatedly viewed and visualized complex images. We observed that the specificity of neural reactivation correlated positively with vivid imagery and with memory for stimulus image details. Moreover, neural reactivation correlated positively with fixation reinstatement, meaning that image-specific eye movements accompanied image-specific patterns of brain activity during visualization. These findings support the conception of mental imagery as a simulation of perception, and provide evidence of the supportive role of eye-movement in neural reactivation.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Half a century ago, Donald Hebb posited that mental imagery is a constructive process that emulates perception. Specifically, Hebb claimed that visual imagery results from the reactivation of neural activity associated with viewing images. He also argued that neural reactivation and imagery benefit from the re-enactment of eye movement patterns that first occurred at viewing (fixation reinstatement). To investigate these claims, we applied multivariate pattern analyses to functional MRI (fMRI) and eye-tracking data collected while healthy human participants repeatedly viewed and visualized complex images. We observed that the specificity of neural reactivation correlated positively with vivid imagery and with memory for stimulus image details. Moreover, neural reactivation correlated positively with fixation reinstatement, meaning that image-specific eye movements accompanied image-specific patterns of brain activity during visualization. These findings support the conception of mental imagery as a simulation of perception, and provide evidence of the supportive role of eye-movement in neural reactivation.

Close

  • doi:10.1093/cercor/bhy014

Close

James A. Brissenden; Sean M. Tobyne; David E. Osher; Emily J. Levin; Mark A. Halko; David C. Somers

Topographic cortico-cerebellar networks revealed by visual attention and working memory Journal Article

In: Current Biology, vol. 28, pp. 3364–3372, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Brissenden2018,
title = {Topographic cortico-cerebellar networks revealed by visual attention and working memory},
author = {James A. Brissenden and Sean M. Tobyne and David E. Osher and Emily J. Levin and Mark A. Halko and David C. Somers},
doi = {10.1016/j.cub.2018.08.059},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Current Biology},
volume = {28},
pages = {3364--3372},
abstract = {Substantial portions of the cerebellum appear to support non-motor functions; however, previous investigations of cerebellar involvement in cognition have revealed only a coarse degree of specificity. Although somatotopic maps have been observed within cerebellum, similar precision within corticocerebellar networks supporting non-motor functions has not previously been reported. Here, we find that human cerebellar lobule VIIb/VIIIa differentially codes key aspects of visuospatial cognition. Ipsilateral visuospatial representations were observed during both a visual working memory and an attentionally demanding visual receptive field-mapping fMRI task paradigm. Moreover, within lobule VIIb/VIIIa, we observed a functional dissociation between spatial coding and visual working memory processing. Visuospatial representations were found in the dorsomedial portion of lobule VIIb/VIIIa, and load dependent visual working memory processing was shifted ventrolaterally. A similar functional gradient for spatial versus load processing was found in posterior parietal cortex. This cerebral cortical organization was well predicted by functional connectivity with spatial and load regions of cerebellar lobule VIIb/VIIIa. Collectively, our findings indicate that recruitment by visuospatial attentional functions within cerebellar lobule VIIb/VIIIa is highly specific. Furthermore, the topographic arrangement of these functions is mirrored in frontal and parietal cortex. These findings motivate a closer examination of cortico-cerebellar functional specialization across a broad range of cognitive domains.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Substantial portions of the cerebellum appear to support non-motor functions; however, previous investigations of cerebellar involvement in cognition have revealed only a coarse degree of specificity. Although somatotopic maps have been observed within cerebellum, similar precision within corticocerebellar networks supporting non-motor functions has not previously been reported. Here, we find that human cerebellar lobule VIIb/VIIIa differentially codes key aspects of visuospatial cognition. Ipsilateral visuospatial representations were observed during both a visual working memory and an attentionally demanding visual receptive field-mapping fMRI task paradigm. Moreover, within lobule VIIb/VIIIa, we observed a functional dissociation between spatial coding and visual working memory processing. Visuospatial representations were found in the dorsomedial portion of lobule VIIb/VIIIa, and load dependent visual working memory processing was shifted ventrolaterally. A similar functional gradient for spatial versus load processing was found in posterior parietal cortex. This cerebral cortical organization was well predicted by functional connectivity with spatial and load regions of cerebellar lobule VIIb/VIIIa. Collectively, our findings indicate that recruitment by visuospatial attentional functions within cerebellar lobule VIIb/VIIIa is highly specific. Furthermore, the topographic arrangement of these functions is mirrored in frontal and parietal cortex. These findings motivate a closer examination of cortico-cerebellar functional specialization across a broad range of cognitive domains.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.cub.2018.08.059

Close

Rotem Broday-Dvir; Shany Grossman; Edna Furman-Haran; Rafael Malach

Quenching of spontaneous fluctuations by attention in human visual cortex Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 171, pp. 84–98, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{BrodayDvir2018,
title = {Quenching of spontaneous fluctuations by attention in human visual cortex},
author = {Rotem Broday-Dvir and Shany Grossman and Edna Furman-Haran and Rafael Malach},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.089},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {171},
pages = {84--98},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {In the absence of a task, the human brain enters a mode of slow spontaneous fluctuations. A fundamental, unresolved question is whether these fluctuations are ongoing and thus persist during task engagement, or alternatively, are quenched and replaced by task-related activations. Here, we examined this issue in the human visual cortex, using fMRI. Participants were asked to either perform a recognition task of randomly appearing face and non-face targets (attended condition) or watch them passively (unattended condition). Importantly, in approximately half of the trials, all sensory stimuli were absent. Our results show that even in the absence of stimuli, spontaneous fluctuations were suppressed by attention. The effect occurred in early visual cortex as well as in fronto-parietal attention network regions. During unattended trials, the activity fluctuations were negatively linked to pupil diameter, arguing against attentional fluctuations as underlying the effect. The results demonstrate that spontaneous fluctuations do not remain unchanged with task performance, but are rather modulated according to behavioral and cognitive demands.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

In the absence of a task, the human brain enters a mode of slow spontaneous fluctuations. A fundamental, unresolved question is whether these fluctuations are ongoing and thus persist during task engagement, or alternatively, are quenched and replaced by task-related activations. Here, we examined this issue in the human visual cortex, using fMRI. Participants were asked to either perform a recognition task of randomly appearing face and non-face targets (attended condition) or watch them passively (unattended condition). Importantly, in approximately half of the trials, all sensory stimuli were absent. Our results show that even in the absence of stimuli, spontaneous fluctuations were suppressed by attention. The effect occurred in early visual cortex as well as in fronto-parietal attention network regions. During unattended trials, the activity fluctuations were negatively linked to pupil diameter, arguing against attentional fluctuations as underlying the effect. The results demonstrate that spontaneous fluctuations do not remain unchanged with task performance, but are rather modulated according to behavioral and cognitive demands.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.089

Close

Daniel Carey; Francesco Caprini; Micah Allen; Antoine Lutti; Nikolaus Weiskopf; Geraint Rees; Martina F. Callaghan; Frederic Dick

Quantitative MRI provides markers of intra-, inter-regional, and age-related differences in young adult cortical microstructure Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 182, pp. 429–440, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Carey2018,
title = {Quantitative MRI provides markers of intra-, inter-regional, and age-related differences in young adult cortical microstructure},
author = {Daniel Carey and Francesco Caprini and Micah Allen and Antoine Lutti and Nikolaus Weiskopf and Geraint Rees and Martina F. Callaghan and Frederic Dick},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.066},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {182},
pages = {429--440},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {Measuring the structural composition of the cortex is critical to understanding typical development, yet few investigations in humans have charted markers in vivo that are sensitive to tissue microstructural attributes. Here, we used a well-validated quantitative MR protocol to measure four parameters (R1, MT, R2* PD*) that differ in their sensitivity to facets of the tissue microstructural environment (R1, MT: myelin, macromolecular content; R2*: myelin, paramagnetic ions, i.e., iron; PD*: free water content). Mapping these parameters across cortical regions in a young adult cohort (18–39 years},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Measuring the structural composition of the cortex is critical to understanding typical development, yet few investigations in humans have charted markers in vivo that are sensitive to tissue microstructural attributes. Here, we used a well-validated quantitative MR protocol to measure four parameters (R1, MT, R2* PD*) that differ in their sensitivity to facets of the tissue microstructural environment (R1, MT: myelin, macromolecular content; R2*: myelin, paramagnetic ions, i.e., iron; PD*: free water content). Mapping these parameters across cortical regions in a young adult cohort (18–39 years

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.066

Close

Natalie Caspari; John T. Arsenault; Rik Vandenberghe; Wim Vanduffel

Functional similarity of medial superior parietal areas for shift-selective attention signals in humans and monkeys Journal Article

In: Cerebral Cortex, vol. 28, no. 6, pp. 2085–2099, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Caspari2018,
title = {Functional similarity of medial superior parietal areas for shift-selective attention signals in humans and monkeys},
author = {Natalie Caspari and John T. Arsenault and Rik Vandenberghe and Wim Vanduffel},
doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhx114},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
volume = {28},
number = {6},
pages = {2085--2099},
abstract = {We continually shift our attention between items in the visual environment. These attention shifts are usually based on task relevance (top-down) or the saliency of a sudden, unexpected stimulus (bottom-up), and are typically followed by goal-directed actions. It could be argued that any species that can covertly shift its focus of attention will rely on similar, evolutionarily conserved neural substrates for processing such shift-signals. To address this possibility, we performed comparative fMRI experiments in humans and monkeys, combining traditional, and novel, data-driven analytical approaches. Specifically, we examined correspondences between monkey and human brain areas activated during covert attention shifts. When " shift " events were compared with " stay " events, the medial (superior) parietal lobe (mSPL) and inferior parietal lobes showed similar shift sensitivities across species, whereas frontal activations were stronger in monkeys. To identify, in a data-driven manner, monkey regions that corresponded with human shift-selective SPL, we used a novel interspecies beta-correlation strategy whereby task-related beta-values were correlated across voxels or regions-of-interest in the 2 species. Monkey medial parietal areas V6/V6A most consistently correlated with shift-selective human mSPL. Our results indicate that both species recruit corresponding, evolutionarily conserved regions within the medial superior parietal lobe for shifting spatial attention.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

We continually shift our attention between items in the visual environment. These attention shifts are usually based on task relevance (top-down) or the saliency of a sudden, unexpected stimulus (bottom-up), and are typically followed by goal-directed actions. It could be argued that any species that can covertly shift its focus of attention will rely on similar, evolutionarily conserved neural substrates for processing such shift-signals. To address this possibility, we performed comparative fMRI experiments in humans and monkeys, combining traditional, and novel, data-driven analytical approaches. Specifically, we examined correspondences between monkey and human brain areas activated during covert attention shifts. When " shift " events were compared with " stay " events, the medial (superior) parietal lobe (mSPL) and inferior parietal lobes showed similar shift sensitivities across species, whereas frontal activations were stronger in monkeys. To identify, in a data-driven manner, monkey regions that corresponded with human shift-selective SPL, we used a novel interspecies beta-correlation strategy whereby task-related beta-values were correlated across voxels or regions-of-interest in the 2 species. Monkey medial parietal areas V6/V6A most consistently correlated with shift-selective human mSPL. Our results indicate that both species recruit corresponding, evolutionarily conserved regions within the medial superior parietal lobe for shifting spatial attention.

Close

  • doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx114

Close

Marshall A. Dalton; Peter Zeidman; Cornelia McCormick; Eleanor A. Maguire

Differentiable processing of objects, associations, and scenes within the hippocampus Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 38, no. 38, pp. 8146–8159, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Dalton2018,
title = {Differentiable processing of objects, associations, and scenes within the hippocampus},
author = {Marshall A. Dalton and Peter Zeidman and Cornelia McCormick and Eleanor A. Maguire},
doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.0263-18.2018},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {38},
number = {38},
pages = {8146--8159},
abstract = {The hippocampus is known to be important for a range of cognitive functions including episodic memory, spatial navigation and future-thinking. Wide agreement on the exact nature of its contribution has proved elusive, with some theories emphasising associative processes and another proposing that scene construction is its primary role. To directly compare these accounts of hippocampal function in human males and females, we devised a novel mental imagery paradigm where different tasks were closely matched for associative processing and mental construction, but either did or did not evoke scene representations, and we combined this with high resolution functional MRI. The results were striking in showing that differentiable parts of the hippocampus, along with distinct cortical regions, were recruited for scene construction or non-scene-evoking associative processing. The contrasting patterns of neural engagement could not be accounted for by differences in eye movements, mnemonic processing or the phenomenology of mental imagery. These results inform conceptual debates in the field by showing that the hippocampus does not seem to favour one type of process over another; it is not a story of exclusivity. Rather, there may be different circuits within the hippocampus, each associated with different cortical inputs, which become engaged depending on the nature of the stimuli and the task at hand. Overall, our findings emphasise the importance of considering the hippocampus as a heterogeneous structure, and that a focus on characterising how specific portions of the hippocampus interact with other brain regions may promote a better understanding of its role in cognition.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The hippocampus is known to be important for a range of cognitive functions including episodic memory, spatial navigation and future-thinking. Wide agreement on the exact nature of its contribution has proved elusive, with some theories emphasising associative processes and another proposing that scene construction is its primary role. To directly compare these accounts of hippocampal function in human males and females, we devised a novel mental imagery paradigm where different tasks were closely matched for associative processing and mental construction, but either did or did not evoke scene representations, and we combined this with high resolution functional MRI. The results were striking in showing that differentiable parts of the hippocampus, along with distinct cortical regions, were recruited for scene construction or non-scene-evoking associative processing. The contrasting patterns of neural engagement could not be accounted for by differences in eye movements, mnemonic processing or the phenomenology of mental imagery. These results inform conceptual debates in the field by showing that the hippocampus does not seem to favour one type of process over another; it is not a story of exclusivity. Rather, there may be different circuits within the hippocampus, each associated with different cortical inputs, which become engaged depending on the nature of the stimuli and the task at hand. Overall, our findings emphasise the importance of considering the hippocampus as a heterogeneous structure, and that a focus on characterising how specific portions of the hippocampus interact with other brain regions may promote a better understanding of its role in cognition.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0263-18.2018

Close

Michelle I. C. Haan; Sonja Wel; Renée M. Visser; H. Steven Scholte; Guido A. Wingen; Merel Kind

The influence of acoustic startle probes on fear learning in humans Journal Article

In: Scientific Reports, vol. 8, pp. 14552, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Haan2018,
title = {The influence of acoustic startle probes on fear learning in humans},
author = {Michelle I. C. Haan and Sonja Wel and Renée M. Visser and H. Steven Scholte and Guido A. Wingen and Merel Kind},
doi = {10.1038/s41598-018-32646-1},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {8},
pages = {14552},
abstract = {Even though human fear-conditioning involves affective learning as well as expectancy learning, most studies assess only one of the two distinct processes. Commonly used read-outs of associative fear learning are the fear-potentiated startle reflex (FPS), pupil dilation and US-expectancy ratings. FPS is thought to reflect the affective aspect of fear learning, while pupil dilation reflects a general arousal response. However, in order to measure FPS, aversively loud acoustic probes are presented during conditioning, which might in itself exert an effect on fear learning. Here we tested the effect of startle probes on fear learning by comparing brain activation (fMRI), pupil dilation and US-expectancy ratings with and without acoustic startle probes within subjects. Regardless of startle probes, fear conditioning resulted in enhanced dACC, insula and ventral striatum activation. Interaction analyses showed that startle probes diminished differential pupil dilation between CS+ and CS− due to increased pupil responses to CS−. A trend significant interaction effect was observed for US-expectancy and amygdala activation. Startle probes affect differential fear learning by impeding safety learning, as measured with pupil dilation, a read-out of the cognitive component of fear learning. However, we observed no significant effect of acoustic startle probes on other measures of fear learning.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Even though human fear-conditioning involves affective learning as well as expectancy learning, most studies assess only one of the two distinct processes. Commonly used read-outs of associative fear learning are the fear-potentiated startle reflex (FPS), pupil dilation and US-expectancy ratings. FPS is thought to reflect the affective aspect of fear learning, while pupil dilation reflects a general arousal response. However, in order to measure FPS, aversively loud acoustic probes are presented during conditioning, which might in itself exert an effect on fear learning. Here we tested the effect of startle probes on fear learning by comparing brain activation (fMRI), pupil dilation and US-expectancy ratings with and without acoustic startle probes within subjects. Regardless of startle probes, fear conditioning resulted in enhanced dACC, insula and ventral striatum activation. Interaction analyses showed that startle probes diminished differential pupil dilation between CS+ and CS− due to increased pupil responses to CS−. A trend significant interaction effect was observed for US-expectancy and amygdala activation. Startle probes affect differential fear learning by impeding safety learning, as measured with pupil dilation, a read-out of the cognitive component of fear learning. However, we observed no significant effect of acoustic startle probes on other measures of fear learning.

Close

  • doi:10.1038/s41598-018-32646-1

Close

Benjamin De Haas; D. Samuel Schwarzkopf

Spatially selective responses to Kanizsa and occlusion stimuli in human visual cortex Journal Article

In: Scientific Reports, vol. 8, pp. 611, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{DeHaas2018,
title = {Spatially selective responses to Kanizsa and occlusion stimuli in human visual cortex},
author = {Benjamin De Haas and D. Samuel Schwarzkopf},
doi = {10.1038/s41598-017-19121-z},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {8},
pages = {611},
publisher = {Springer US},
abstract = {Early visual cortex responds to illusory contours in which abutting lines or collinear edges imply the presence of an occluding surface, as well as to occluded parts of an object. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and population receptive field (pRF) analysis to map retinotopic responses in early visual cortex using bar stimuli defined by illusory contours, occluded parts of a bar, or subtle luminance contrast. All conditions produced retinotopic responses in early visual field maps even though signal-to-noise ratios were very low. We found that signal-to-noise ratios and coherence with independent high-contrast mapping data increased from V1 to V2 to V3. Moreover, we found no differences of signal-to-noise ratios or pRF sizes between the low-contrast luminance and illusion conditions. We propose that all three conditions mapped spatial attention to the bar location rather than activations specifically related to illusory contours or occlusion.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Early visual cortex responds to illusory contours in which abutting lines or collinear edges imply the presence of an occluding surface, as well as to occluded parts of an object. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and population receptive field (pRF) analysis to map retinotopic responses in early visual cortex using bar stimuli defined by illusory contours, occluded parts of a bar, or subtle luminance contrast. All conditions produced retinotopic responses in early visual field maps even though signal-to-noise ratios were very low. We found that signal-to-noise ratios and coherence with independent high-contrast mapping data increased from V1 to V2 to V3. Moreover, we found no differences of signal-to-noise ratios or pRF sizes between the low-contrast luminance and illusion conditions. We propose that all three conditions mapped spatial attention to the bar location rather than activations specifically related to illusory contours or occlusion.

Close

  • doi:10.1038/s41598-017-19121-z

Close

Katharina Dobs; Johannes Schultz; Isabelle Bülthoff; Justin L. Gardner

Task-dependent enhancement of facial expression and identity representations in human cortex Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 172, pp. 689–702, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Dobs2018,
title = {Task-dependent enhancement of facial expression and identity representations in human cortex},
author = {Katharina Dobs and Johannes Schultz and Isabelle Bülthoff and Justin L. Gardner},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.013},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {172},
pages = {689--702},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {What cortical mechanisms allow humans to easily discern the expression or identity of a face? Subjects detected changes in expression or identity of a stream of dynamic faces while we measured BOLD responses from topographically and functionally defined areas throughout the visual hierarchy. Responses in dorsal areas increased during the expression task, whereas responses in ventral areas increased during the identity task, consistent with previous studies. Similar to ventral areas, early visual areas showed increased activity during the identity task. If visual responses are weighted by perceptual mechanisms according to their magnitude, these increased responses would lead to improved attentional selection of the task-appropriate facial aspect. Alternatively, increased responses could be a signature of a sensitivity enhancement mechanism that improves representations of the attended facial aspect. Consistent with the latter sensitivity enhancement mechanism, attending to expression led to enhanced decoding of exemplars of expression both in early visual and dorsal areas relative to attending identity. Similarly, decoding identity exemplars when attending to identity was improved in dorsal and ventral areas. We conclude that attending to expression or identity of dynamic faces is associated with increased selectivity in representations consistent with sensitivity enhancement.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

What cortical mechanisms allow humans to easily discern the expression or identity of a face? Subjects detected changes in expression or identity of a stream of dynamic faces while we measured BOLD responses from topographically and functionally defined areas throughout the visual hierarchy. Responses in dorsal areas increased during the expression task, whereas responses in ventral areas increased during the identity task, consistent with previous studies. Similar to ventral areas, early visual areas showed increased activity during the identity task. If visual responses are weighted by perceptual mechanisms according to their magnitude, these increased responses would lead to improved attentional selection of the task-appropriate facial aspect. Alternatively, increased responses could be a signature of a sensitivity enhancement mechanism that improves representations of the attended facial aspect. Consistent with the latter sensitivity enhancement mechanism, attending to expression led to enhanced decoding of exemplars of expression both in early visual and dorsal areas relative to attending identity. Similarly, decoding identity exemplars when attending to identity was improved in dorsal and ventral areas. We conclude that attending to expression or identity of dynamic faces is associated with increased selectivity in representations consistent with sensitivity enhancement.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.013

Close

Laura Dugué; Elisha P. Merriam; David J. Heeger; Marisa Carrasco

Specific visual subregions of TPJ mediate reorienting of spatial attention Journal Article

In: Cerebral Cortex, vol. 28, no. 7, pp. 2375–2390, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Dugue2018,
title = {Specific visual subregions of TPJ mediate reorienting of spatial attention},
author = {Laura Dugué and Elisha P. Merriam and David J. Heeger and Marisa Carrasco},
doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhx140},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
volume = {28},
number = {7},
pages = {2375--2390},
abstract = {The temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) has been associated with various cognitive and social functions, and is critical for attentional reorienting. Attention affects early visual processing. Neuroimaging studies dealing with such processes have thus far concentrated on striate and extrastriate areas. Here, we investigated whether attention orienting or reorienting modulate activity in visually driven TPJ subregions. For each observer we identified 3 visually responsive subregions within TPJ: 2 bilateral (vTPJ ant and vTPJ post) and 1 right lateralized (vTPJ cent). Cortical activity in these subregions was measured using fMRI while observers performed a 2-alternative forced-choice orientation discrimination task. Covert spatial endogenous (voluntary) or exogenous (involuntary) attention was manipulated using either a central or a peripheral cue with task, stimuli and observers constant. Both endogenous and exogenous attention increased activity for invalidly cued trials in right vTPJ post ; only endogenous attention increased activity for invalidly cued trials in left vTPJ post and in right vTPJ cent ; and neither type of attention modulated either right or left vTPJ ant . These results demonstrate that vTPJ post and vTPJ cent mediate the reorientation of covert attention to task relevant stimuli, thus playing a critical role in visual attention. These findings reveal a differential reorienting cortical response after observers' attention has been oriented to a given location voluntarily or involuntarily.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) has been associated with various cognitive and social functions, and is critical for attentional reorienting. Attention affects early visual processing. Neuroimaging studies dealing with such processes have thus far concentrated on striate and extrastriate areas. Here, we investigated whether attention orienting or reorienting modulate activity in visually driven TPJ subregions. For each observer we identified 3 visually responsive subregions within TPJ: 2 bilateral (vTPJ ant and vTPJ post) and 1 right lateralized (vTPJ cent). Cortical activity in these subregions was measured using fMRI while observers performed a 2-alternative forced-choice orientation discrimination task. Covert spatial endogenous (voluntary) or exogenous (involuntary) attention was manipulated using either a central or a peripheral cue with task, stimuli and observers constant. Both endogenous and exogenous attention increased activity for invalidly cued trials in right vTPJ post ; only endogenous attention increased activity for invalidly cued trials in left vTPJ post and in right vTPJ cent ; and neither type of attention modulated either right or left vTPJ ant . These results demonstrate that vTPJ post and vTPJ cent mediate the reorientation of covert attention to task relevant stimuli, thus playing a critical role in visual attention. These findings reveal a differential reorienting cortical response after observers' attention has been oriented to a given location voluntarily or involuntarily.

Close

  • doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx140

Close

Katherine Duncan; Bradley B. Doll; Nathaniel D. Daw; Daphna Shohamy

More than the sum of its parts: A role for the hippocampus in configural reinforcement learning Journal Article

In: Neuron, vol. 98, no. 3, pp. 645–657.e6, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Duncan2018,
title = {More than the sum of its parts: A role for the hippocampus in configural reinforcement learning},
author = {Katherine Duncan and Bradley B. Doll and Nathaniel D. Daw and Daphna Shohamy},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.042},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Neuron},
volume = {98},
number = {3},
pages = {645--657.e6},
publisher = {Elsevier Inc.},
abstract = {People often perceive configurations rather than the elements they comprise, a bias that may emerge because configurations often predict outcomes. But how does the brain learn to associate configurations with outcomes and how does this learning differ from learning about individual elements? We combined behavior, reinforcement learning models, and functional imaging to understand how people learn to associate configurations of cues with outcomes. We found that configural learning depended on the relative predictive strength of elements versus configurations and was related to both the strength of BOLD activity and patterns of BOLD activity in the hippocampus. Configural learning was further related to functional connectivity between the hippocampus and nucleus accumbens. Moreover, configural learning was associated with flexible knowledge about associations and differential eye movements during choice. Together, this suggests that configural learning is associated with a distinct computational, cognitive, and neural profile that is well suited to support flexible and adaptive behavior. Duncan et al. investigate how people learn to predict outcomes using cue configurations. They show that configural learning is characterized by unique computational, behavioral, and neural signatures, including hippocampal activity, interactions between the hippocampus and striatum, and enhanced flexible knowledge.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

People often perceive configurations rather than the elements they comprise, a bias that may emerge because configurations often predict outcomes. But how does the brain learn to associate configurations with outcomes and how does this learning differ from learning about individual elements? We combined behavior, reinforcement learning models, and functional imaging to understand how people learn to associate configurations of cues with outcomes. We found that configural learning depended on the relative predictive strength of elements versus configurations and was related to both the strength of BOLD activity and patterns of BOLD activity in the hippocampus. Configural learning was further related to functional connectivity between the hippocampus and nucleus accumbens. Moreover, configural learning was associated with flexible knowledge about associations and differential eye movements during choice. Together, this suggests that configural learning is associated with a distinct computational, cognitive, and neural profile that is well suited to support flexible and adaptive behavior. Duncan et al. investigate how people learn to predict outcomes using cue configurations. They show that configural learning is characterized by unique computational, behavioral, and neural signatures, including hippocampal activity, interactions between the hippocampus and striatum, and enhanced flexible knowledge.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.042

Close

Kathleen A. Garrison; Stephanie S. O'malley; Ralitza Gueorguieva; Suchitra Krishnan-Sarin

A fMRI study on the impact of advertising for flavored e-cigarettes on susceptible young adults Journal Article

In: Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 186, pp. 233–241, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Garrison2018,
title = {A fMRI study on the impact of advertising for flavored e-cigarettes on susceptible young adults},
author = {Kathleen A. Garrison and Stephanie S. O'malley and Ralitza Gueorguieva and Suchitra Krishnan-Sarin},
doi = {10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.01.026},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Drug and Alcohol Dependence},
volume = {186},
pages = {233--241},
abstract = {Background: E-cigarettes are sold in flavors such as "skittles," "strawberrylicious," and "juicy fruit," and no restrictions are in place on marketing e-cigarettes to youth. Sweets/fruits depicted in e-cigarette advertisements may increase their appeal to youth and interfere with health warnings. This study tested a brain biomarker of product preference for sweet/fruit versus tobacco flavor e-cigarettes, and whether advertising for flavors interfered with warning labels. Methods: Participants (N = 26) were college-age young adults who had tried an e-cigarette and were susceptible to future e-cigarette use. They viewed advertisements in fMRI for sweet/fruit and tobacco flavor e-cigarettes, menthol and regular cigarettes, and control images of sweets/fruits/mints with no tobacco product. Cue-reactivity was measured in the nucleus accumbens, a brain biomarker of product preference. Advertisements randomly contained warning labels, and recognition of health warnings was tested post-scan. Visual attention was measured using eye-tracking. Results: There was a significant effect of e-cigarette condition (sweet/tobacco/control) on nucleus accumbens activity, that was not found for cigarette condition (menthol/regular/control). Nucleus accumbens activity was greater for sweet/fruit versus tobacco flavor e-cigarette advertisements and did not differ compared with control images of sweets and fruits. Greater nucleus accumbens activity was correlated with poorer memory for health warnings. Conclusions: These and exploratory eye-tracking findings suggest that advertising for sweet/fruit flavors may increase positive associations with e-cigarettes and/or override negative associations with tobacco, and interfere with health warnings, suggesting that one way to reduce the appeal of e-cigarettes to youth and educate youth about e-cigarette health risks is to regulate advertising for flavors.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Background: E-cigarettes are sold in flavors such as "skittles," "strawberrylicious," and "juicy fruit," and no restrictions are in place on marketing e-cigarettes to youth. Sweets/fruits depicted in e-cigarette advertisements may increase their appeal to youth and interfere with health warnings. This study tested a brain biomarker of product preference for sweet/fruit versus tobacco flavor e-cigarettes, and whether advertising for flavors interfered with warning labels. Methods: Participants (N = 26) were college-age young adults who had tried an e-cigarette and were susceptible to future e-cigarette use. They viewed advertisements in fMRI for sweet/fruit and tobacco flavor e-cigarettes, menthol and regular cigarettes, and control images of sweets/fruits/mints with no tobacco product. Cue-reactivity was measured in the nucleus accumbens, a brain biomarker of product preference. Advertisements randomly contained warning labels, and recognition of health warnings was tested post-scan. Visual attention was measured using eye-tracking. Results: There was a significant effect of e-cigarette condition (sweet/tobacco/control) on nucleus accumbens activity, that was not found for cigarette condition (menthol/regular/control). Nucleus accumbens activity was greater for sweet/fruit versus tobacco flavor e-cigarette advertisements and did not differ compared with control images of sweets and fruits. Greater nucleus accumbens activity was correlated with poorer memory for health warnings. Conclusions: These and exploratory eye-tracking findings suggest that advertising for sweet/fruit flavors may increase positive associations with e-cigarettes and/or override negative associations with tobacco, and interfere with health warnings, suggesting that one way to reduce the appeal of e-cigarettes to youth and educate youth about e-cigarette health risks is to regulate advertising for flavors.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.01.026

Close

Detre A. Godinez; Daniel S. Lumian; Tanisha Crosby-Attipoe; Ana M. Bedacarratz; Paree Zarolia; Kateri McRae

Overlapping and distinct neural correlates of imitating and opposing facial movements Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 166, pp. 239–246, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Godinez2018,
title = {Overlapping and distinct neural correlates of imitating and opposing facial movements},
author = {Detre A. Godinez and Daniel S. Lumian and Tanisha Crosby-Attipoe and Ana M. Bedacarratz and Paree Zarolia and Kateri McRae},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.023},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {166},
pages = {239--246},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {Previous studies have demonstrated that imitating a face can be relatively automatic and reflexive. In contrast, opposing facial expressions may require engaging flexible, cognitive control. However, few studies have examined the degree to which imitation and opposition of facial movements recruit overlapping and distinct neural regions. Furthermore, little work has examined whether opposition and imitation of facial movements differ between emotional and averted eye gaze facial expressions. This study utilized a novel task with 40 participants to compare passive viewing, imitation and opposition of emotional faces looking forward and neutral faces with averted eye gaze [(3: Look, Imitate, Oppose) x (2: Emotion, Averted Eye)]. Imitation and opposition of both types of facial movements elicited overlapping activation in frontal, premotor, superior temporal and anterior intraparietal regions. These regions are recruited during cognitive control, face processing and mirroring tasks. For both emotional and averted eye gaze photos, opposition engaged the superior frontal gyrus, superior temporal sulcus and the anterior intraparietal sulcus to a greater extent compared to imitation. Finally, stimulus type and instruction interacted, such that for the eye gaze condition only, greater activation was observed in the dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC) during opposition compared to imitation, while no significant dACC differences were observed for the emotional expression conditions, which instead showed significantly greater activation in the middle and frontal pole. Overall these results showed significant overlap between imitation and opposition, as well as increased activation of these regions to generate an opposing facial movement relative to imitating.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Previous studies have demonstrated that imitating a face can be relatively automatic and reflexive. In contrast, opposing facial expressions may require engaging flexible, cognitive control. However, few studies have examined the degree to which imitation and opposition of facial movements recruit overlapping and distinct neural regions. Furthermore, little work has examined whether opposition and imitation of facial movements differ between emotional and averted eye gaze facial expressions. This study utilized a novel task with 40 participants to compare passive viewing, imitation and opposition of emotional faces looking forward and neutral faces with averted eye gaze [(3: Look, Imitate, Oppose) x (2: Emotion, Averted Eye)]. Imitation and opposition of both types of facial movements elicited overlapping activation in frontal, premotor, superior temporal and anterior intraparietal regions. These regions are recruited during cognitive control, face processing and mirroring tasks. For both emotional and averted eye gaze photos, opposition engaged the superior frontal gyrus, superior temporal sulcus and the anterior intraparietal sulcus to a greater extent compared to imitation. Finally, stimulus type and instruction interacted, such that for the eye gaze condition only, greater activation was observed in the dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC) during opposition compared to imitation, while no significant dACC differences were observed for the emotional expression conditions, which instead showed significantly greater activation in the middle and frontal pole. Overall these results showed significant overlap between imitation and opposition, as well as increased activation of these regions to generate an opposing facial movement relative to imitating.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.023

Close

Christoph Naegeli; Thomas Zeffiro; Marco Piccirelli; Assia Jaillard; Anina Weilenmann; Katayun Hassanpour; Matthis Schick; Michael Rufer; Scott P. Orr; Christoph Mueller-Pfeiffer

Locus coeruleus activity mediates hyperresponsiveness in posttraumatic stress disorder Journal Article

In: Biological Psychiatry, vol. 83, no. 3, pp. 254–262, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Naegeli2018,
title = {Locus coeruleus activity mediates hyperresponsiveness in posttraumatic stress disorder},
author = {Christoph Naegeli and Thomas Zeffiro and Marco Piccirelli and Assia Jaillard and Anina Weilenmann and Katayun Hassanpour and Matthis Schick and Michael Rufer and Scott P. Orr and Christoph Mueller-Pfeiffer},
doi = {10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.08.021},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Biological Psychiatry},
volume = {83},
number = {3},
pages = {254--262},
abstract = {Background: Patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are hyperresponsive to unexpected or potentially threatening environmental stimuli. Research in lower animals and humans suggests that sensitization of the locus coeruleus–norepinephrine system may underlie behavioral and autonomic hyperresponsiveness in PTSD. However, direct evidence linking locus coeruleus system hyperactivity to PTSD hyperresponsiveness is sparse. Methods: Psychophysiological recording and functional magnetic resonance imaging were used during passive listening to brief, 95-dB sound pressure level, white noise bursts presented intermittently to determine whether behavioral and autonomic hyperresponsiveness to sudden sounds in PTSD is associated with locus coeruleus hyperresponsiveness. Results: Participants with PTSD (n = 28) showed more eye-blink reflexes and larger heart rate, skin conductance, and pupil area responses to loud sounds (multivariate p =.007) compared with trauma-exposed participants without PTSD (n = 26). PTSD participants exhibited larger responses in locus coeruleus (t = 2.60, region of interest familywise error corrected), intraparietal sulcus, caudal dorsal premotor cortex, and cerebellar lobule VI (t ≥ 4.18, whole-brain familywise error corrected). Caudal dorsal premotor cortex activity was associated with both psychophysiological response magnitude and levels of exaggerated startle responses in daily life in PTSD participants (t ≥ 4.39, whole-brain familywise error corrected). Conclusions: Behavioral and autonomic hyperresponsiveness in PTSD may arise from a hyperactive alerting/orienting system in which processes related to attention and motor preparation localized to lateral premotor cortex, intraparietal sulcus, and posterior superior cerebellar cortex are modulated by atypically high phasic noradrenergic influences originating in the locus coeruleus.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Background: Patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) are hyperresponsive to unexpected or potentially threatening environmental stimuli. Research in lower animals and humans suggests that sensitization of the locus coeruleus–norepinephrine system may underlie behavioral and autonomic hyperresponsiveness in PTSD. However, direct evidence linking locus coeruleus system hyperactivity to PTSD hyperresponsiveness is sparse. Methods: Psychophysiological recording and functional magnetic resonance imaging were used during passive listening to brief, 95-dB sound pressure level, white noise bursts presented intermittently to determine whether behavioral and autonomic hyperresponsiveness to sudden sounds in PTSD is associated with locus coeruleus hyperresponsiveness. Results: Participants with PTSD (n = 28) showed more eye-blink reflexes and larger heart rate, skin conductance, and pupil area responses to loud sounds (multivariate p =.007) compared with trauma-exposed participants without PTSD (n = 26). PTSD participants exhibited larger responses in locus coeruleus (t = 2.60, region of interest familywise error corrected), intraparietal sulcus, caudal dorsal premotor cortex, and cerebellar lobule VI (t ≥ 4.18, whole-brain familywise error corrected). Caudal dorsal premotor cortex activity was associated with both psychophysiological response magnitude and levels of exaggerated startle responses in daily life in PTSD participants (t ≥ 4.39, whole-brain familywise error corrected). Conclusions: Behavioral and autonomic hyperresponsiveness in PTSD may arise from a hyperactive alerting/orienting system in which processes related to attention and motor preparation localized to lateral premotor cortex, intraparietal sulcus, and posterior superior cerebellar cortex are modulated by atypically high phasic noradrenergic influences originating in the locus coeruleus.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.08.021

Close

Vaidehi S. Natu; Jesse Gomez; Kalanit Grill-Spector; Brianna Jeska; Michael Barnett

Development differentially sculpts receptive fields across early and high-level human visual cortex Journal Article

In: Nature Communications, vol. 9, pp. 788, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Natu2018,
title = {Development differentially sculpts receptive fields across early and high-level human visual cortex},
author = {Vaidehi S. Natu and Jesse Gomez and Kalanit Grill-Spector and Brianna Jeska and Michael Barnett},
doi = {10.1038/s41467-018-03166-3},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Nature Communications},
volume = {9},
pages = {788},
publisher = {Springer US},
abstract = {Receptive fields (RFs) processing information in restricted parts of the visual field are a key property of visual system neurons. However, how RFs develop in humans is unknown. Using fMRI and population receptive field (pRF) modeling in children and adults, we determine where and how pRFs develop across the ventral visual stream. Here we report that pRF properties in visual field maps, from the first visual area, V1, through the first ventro-occipital area, VO1, are adult-like by age 5. However, pRF properties in face-selective and character- selective regions develop into adulthood, increasing the foveal coverage bias for faces in the right hemisphere and words in the left hemisphere. Eye-tracking indicates that pRF changes are related to changing fixation patterns on words and faces across development. These findings suggest a link between face and word viewing behavior and the differential development of pRFs across visual cortex, potentially due to competition on foveal coverage.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Receptive fields (RFs) processing information in restricted parts of the visual field are a key property of visual system neurons. However, how RFs develop in humans is unknown. Using fMRI and population receptive field (pRF) modeling in children and adults, we determine where and how pRFs develop across the ventral visual stream. Here we report that pRF properties in visual field maps, from the first visual area, V1, through the first ventro-occipital area, VO1, are adult-like by age 5. However, pRF properties in face-selective and character- selective regions develop into adulthood, increasing the foveal coverage bias for faces in the right hemisphere and words in the left hemisphere. Eye-tracking indicates that pRF changes are related to changing fixation patterns on words and faces across development. These findings suggest a link between face and word viewing behavior and the differential development of pRFs across visual cortex, potentially due to competition on foveal coverage.

Close

  • doi:10.1038/s41467-018-03166-3

Close

Matthias Nau; Tobias Navarro Schröder; Jacob L. S. Bellmund; Christian F. Doeller

Hexadirectional coding of visual space in human entorhinal cortex Journal Article

In: Nature Neuroscience, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 188–190, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Nau2018,
title = {Hexadirectional coding of visual space in human entorhinal cortex},
author = {Matthias Nau and Tobias Navarro Schröder and Jacob L. S. Bellmund and Christian F. Doeller},
doi = {10.1038/s41593-017-0050-8},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Nature Neuroscience},
volume = {21},
number = {2},
pages = {188--190},
publisher = {Springer US},
abstract = {Entorhinal grid cells map the local environment, but their involvement beyond spatial navigation remains elusive. We examined human functional MRI responses during a highly controlled visual tracking task and show that entorhinal cortex exhibited a sixfold rotationally symmetric signal encoding gaze direction. Our results provide evidence for a grid-like entorhinal code for visual space and suggest a more general role of the entorhinal grid system in coding information along continuous dimensions.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Entorhinal grid cells map the local environment, but their involvement beyond spatial navigation remains elusive. We examined human functional MRI responses during a highly controlled visual tracking task and show that entorhinal cortex exhibited a sixfold rotationally symmetric signal encoding gaze direction. Our results provide evidence for a grid-like entorhinal code for visual space and suggest a more general role of the entorhinal grid system in coding information along continuous dimensions.

Close

  • doi:10.1038/s41593-017-0050-8

Close

Claire K. Naughtin; Jason B. Mattingley; Angela D. Bender; Paul E. Dux

Decoding early and late cortical contributions to individuation of attended and unattended objects Journal Article

In: Cortex, vol. 99, pp. 45–54, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Naughtin2018,
title = {Decoding early and late cortical contributions to individuation of attended and unattended objects},
author = {Claire K. Naughtin and Jason B. Mattingley and Angela D. Bender and Paul E. Dux},
doi = {10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.013},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Cortex},
volume = {99},
pages = {45--54},
abstract = {To isolate a visual stimulus as a unique object with a specific spatial location and time of occurrence, it is necessary to first register (individuate) the stimulus as a distinct perceptual entity. Recent investigations into the neural substrates of object individuation have suggested it is subserved by a distributed neural network, but previous manipulations of individuation load have introduced extraneous visual confounds, which might have yielded ambiguous findings, particularly in early cortical areas. Furthermore, while it has been assumed that selective attention is required for object individuation, there is no definitive evidence on the brain regions recruited for attended and ignored objects. Here we addressed these issues by combining functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with a novel object-enumeration paradigm in which to-be-individuated objects were defined by illusory contours, such that the physical elements of the display remained constant across individuation conditions. Multi-voxel pattern analyses revealed that attended objects modulated patterns of activity in early visual cortex, as well as frontal and parietal brain areas, as a function of object-individuation load. These findings suggest that object indi-viduation recruits both early and later cortical areas, consistent with theoretical accounts proposing that this operation acts at the junction of feed-forward and feedback processing stages in visual analysis. We also found dissociations between brain regions involved in individuation of attended and unattended objects, suggesting that voluntary spatial attention influences the brain regions recruited for this process.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

To isolate a visual stimulus as a unique object with a specific spatial location and time of occurrence, it is necessary to first register (individuate) the stimulus as a distinct perceptual entity. Recent investigations into the neural substrates of object individuation have suggested it is subserved by a distributed neural network, but previous manipulations of individuation load have introduced extraneous visual confounds, which might have yielded ambiguous findings, particularly in early cortical areas. Furthermore, while it has been assumed that selective attention is required for object individuation, there is no definitive evidence on the brain regions recruited for attended and ignored objects. Here we addressed these issues by combining functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with a novel object-enumeration paradigm in which to-be-individuated objects were defined by illusory contours, such that the physical elements of the display remained constant across individuation conditions. Multi-voxel pattern analyses revealed that attended objects modulated patterns of activity in early visual cortex, as well as frontal and parietal brain areas, as a function of object-individuation load. These findings suggest that object indi-viduation recruits both early and later cortical areas, consistent with theoretical accounts proposing that this operation acts at the junction of feed-forward and feedback processing stages in visual analysis. We also found dissociations between brain regions involved in individuation of attended and unattended objects, suggesting that voluntary spatial attention influences the brain regions recruited for this process.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.013

Close

Akitoshi Ogawa; Atsushi Ueshima; Keigo Inukai; Tatsuya Kameda

Deciding for others as a neutral party recruits risk-neutral perspective-taking: Model-based behavioral and fMRI experiments Journal Article

In: Scientific Reports, vol. 8, pp. 12857, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Ogawa2018,
title = {Deciding for others as a neutral party recruits risk-neutral perspective-taking: Model-based behavioral and fMRI experiments},
author = {Akitoshi Ogawa and Atsushi Ueshima and Keigo Inukai and Tatsuya Kameda},
doi = {10.1038/s41598-018-31308-6},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {8},
pages = {12857},
abstract = {Risky decision making for others is ubiquitous in our societies. Whereas financial decision making for oneself induces strong concern about the worst outcome (maximin concern) as well as the expected value, behavioral and neural characteristics of decision making for others are less well understood. We conducted behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments to examine the neurocognitive underpinnings of risky decisions for an anonymous other, using decisions for self as a benchmark. We show that, although the maximin concern affected both types of decisions equally strongly, decision making for others recruited a more risk-neutral computational mechanism than decision making for self. Specifically, participants exhibited more balanced information search when choosing a risky option for others. Activity of right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ, associated with cognitive perspective taking) was parametrically modulated by options' expected values in decisions for others, and by the minimum amounts in decisions for self. Furthermore, individual differences in self-reported empathic concern modified these attentional and neural processes. Overall, these results indicate that the typical maximin concern is attenuated in a risk-neutral direction in decisions for others as compared to self. We conjecture that, given others' diverse preferences, deciding as a neutral party may cognitively recruit such risk-neutrality.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Risky decision making for others is ubiquitous in our societies. Whereas financial decision making for oneself induces strong concern about the worst outcome (maximin concern) as well as the expected value, behavioral and neural characteristics of decision making for others are less well understood. We conducted behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments to examine the neurocognitive underpinnings of risky decisions for an anonymous other, using decisions for self as a benchmark. We show that, although the maximin concern affected both types of decisions equally strongly, decision making for others recruited a more risk-neutral computational mechanism than decision making for self. Specifically, participants exhibited more balanced information search when choosing a risky option for others. Activity of right temporoparietal junction (rTPJ, associated with cognitive perspective taking) was parametrically modulated by options' expected values in decisions for others, and by the minimum amounts in decisions for self. Furthermore, individual differences in self-reported empathic concern modified these attentional and neural processes. Overall, these results indicate that the typical maximin concern is attenuated in a risk-neutral direction in decisions for others as compared to self. We conjecture that, given others' diverse preferences, deciding as a neutral party may cognitively recruit such risk-neutrality.

Close

  • doi:10.1038/s41598-018-31308-6

Close

Tanya Orlov; Ehud Zohary

Object representations in human visual cortex formed through temporal integration of dynamic partial shape views Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 38, no. 3, pp. 659–678, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Orlov2018,
title = {Object representations in human visual cortex formed through temporal integration of dynamic partial shape views},
author = {Tanya Orlov and Ehud Zohary},
doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.1318-17.2017},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {38},
number = {3},
pages = {659--678},
abstract = {We typically recognize visual objects, by utilizing the spatial layout of their parts, simultaneously present on the retina. Thus, shape extraction is based on integration of the relevant retinal information over space. The lateral occipital complex (LOC) can faithfully represent shape in such conditions. However, sometimes, integration over time is required to determine object shape. To study shape extraction through temporal integration of successive partial-shape views, we presented human participants (both men and women) with artificial shapes that moved behind a narrow vertical or horizontal slit. Only a tiny fraction of the shape was visible at any instant, at the sameretinal location. Yet, observers perceived a coherent whole shape instead of a jumbled pattern.Using fMRI and multivoxel-pattern analysis we searched for brain regions that encode temporally-integrated shape identity. We further required that the representation of shape should be invariant to changes in the slit-orientation. We show that slit-invariant shape information is most accurate in LOC. Importantly, the slit-invariant shape representations matched the conventional whole-shape representations assessed during full-image runs. Moreover, when the same slit-dependent shape-slivers were shuffled, thereby preventing their spatiotemporal integration, slit-invariant shape information was dramatically reduced. The slit-invariant representation of the various shapes also mirrored the structure of shape perceptual space, as assessed by perceptual similarity-judgment tests. Thus, LOC is likely to mediate temporal integration of slit-dependent shape-views, generating a slit-invariant whole-shape percept. These findings provide strong evidence for a global encoding of shape in LOC, regardless of integration processes required to generate the shape percept.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

We typically recognize visual objects, by utilizing the spatial layout of their parts, simultaneously present on the retina. Thus, shape extraction is based on integration of the relevant retinal information over space. The lateral occipital complex (LOC) can faithfully represent shape in such conditions. However, sometimes, integration over time is required to determine object shape. To study shape extraction through temporal integration of successive partial-shape views, we presented human participants (both men and women) with artificial shapes that moved behind a narrow vertical or horizontal slit. Only a tiny fraction of the shape was visible at any instant, at the sameretinal location. Yet, observers perceived a coherent whole shape instead of a jumbled pattern.Using fMRI and multivoxel-pattern analysis we searched for brain regions that encode temporally-integrated shape identity. We further required that the representation of shape should be invariant to changes in the slit-orientation. We show that slit-invariant shape information is most accurate in LOC. Importantly, the slit-invariant shape representations matched the conventional whole-shape representations assessed during full-image runs. Moreover, when the same slit-dependent shape-slivers were shuffled, thereby preventing their spatiotemporal integration, slit-invariant shape information was dramatically reduced. The slit-invariant representation of the various shapes also mirrored the structure of shape perceptual space, as assessed by perceptual similarity-judgment tests. Thus, LOC is likely to mediate temporal integration of slit-dependent shape-views, generating a slit-invariant whole-shape percept. These findings provide strong evidence for a global encoding of shape in LOC, regardless of integration processes required to generate the shape percept.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1318-17.2017

Close

Alexandra Papadopoulos; Francesco Sforazzini; Gary F. Egan; Sharna D. Jamadar

Functional subdivisions within the human intraparietal sulcus are involved in visuospatial transformation in a non-context-dependent manner Journal Article

In: Human Brain Mapping, vol. 39, no. 1, pp. 354–368, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Papadopoulos2018,
title = {Functional subdivisions within the human intraparietal sulcus are involved in visuospatial transformation in a non-context-dependent manner},
author = {Alexandra Papadopoulos and Francesco Sforazzini and Gary F. Egan and Sharna D. Jamadar},
doi = {10.1002/hbm.23847},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Human Brain Mapping},
volume = {39},
number = {1},
pages = {354--368},
abstract = {Object-based visuospatial transformation is important for the ability to interact with the world and the people and objects within it. In this preliminary investigation, we hypothesized that object-based visuospatial transformation is a unitary process invoked regardless of current context and is localized to the intraparietal sulcus. Participants (n = 14) performed both antisaccade and mental rotation tasks while scanned using fMRI. A statistical conjunction confirmed that both tasks activated the intraparietal sulcus. Statistical parametric anatomical mapping determined that the statistical conjunction was localized to intraparietal sulcus subregions hIP2 and hIP3. A Gaussian naive Bayes classifier confirmed that the conjunction in region hIP3 was indistinguishable between tasks. The results provide evidence that object-based visuospatial transformation is a domain-general process that is invoked regardless of current context. Our results are consistent with the modular model of the posterior parietal cortex and the distinct cytoarchitectonic, structural, and functional connectivity profiles of the subregions in the intraparietal sulcus.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Object-based visuospatial transformation is important for the ability to interact with the world and the people and objects within it. In this preliminary investigation, we hypothesized that object-based visuospatial transformation is a unitary process invoked regardless of current context and is localized to the intraparietal sulcus. Participants (n = 14) performed both antisaccade and mental rotation tasks while scanned using fMRI. A statistical conjunction confirmed that both tasks activated the intraparietal sulcus. Statistical parametric anatomical mapping determined that the statistical conjunction was localized to intraparietal sulcus subregions hIP2 and hIP3. A Gaussian naive Bayes classifier confirmed that the conjunction in region hIP3 was indistinguishable between tasks. The results provide evidence that object-based visuospatial transformation is a domain-general process that is invoked regardless of current context. Our results are consistent with the modular model of the posterior parietal cortex and the distinct cytoarchitectonic, structural, and functional connectivity profiles of the subregions in the intraparietal sulcus.

Close

  • doi:10.1002/hbm.23847

Close

Andrew S. Persichetti; Daniel D. Dilks

Dissociable neural systems for recognizing places and navigating through them Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 38, no. 48, pp. 10295–10304, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Persichetti2018,
title = {Dissociable neural systems for recognizing places and navigating through them},
author = {Andrew S. Persichetti and Daniel D. Dilks},
doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.1200-18.2018},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {38},
number = {48},
pages = {10295--10304},
abstract = {When entering an environment, we can use the present visual information from the scene to either recognize the kind ofplace it is (e.g., a kitchen or a bedroom) or navigate through it. Here we directly test the hypothesis that these two processes, what we call “scene categorization” and “visually-guided navigation”, are supported by dissociable neural systems. Specifically, we manipulated task demands by asking human participants (male and female) to perform a scene categorization, visually-guided navigation, and baseline task on images of scenes, and measured both the average univariate responses and multivariate spatial pattern of responses within two scene-selective cortical regions, the parahippocampal place area (PPA) and occipital place area (OPA), hypothesized to be separably involved in scene categorization and visually-guided navigation, respectively. As predicted, in the univariate analysis, PPA responded significantly more during the categorization task than during both the navigation and baseline tasks, whereas OPA showed the complete opposite pattern. Similarly, in the multivariate analysis, a linear support vector machine achieved above-chance classification for the categorization task, but not the navigation task in PPA. By contrast, above-chance classification was achieved for both the navigation and categorization tasks in OPA. However, above-chance classification for both tasks was also found in early visual cortex and hence not specific to OPA, suggesting that the spatial patterns of responses in OPA are merely inherited from early vision, and thus may be epiphenomenal to behavior. Together, these results are evidence for dissociable neural systems involved in recognizing places and navigating through them.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

When entering an environment, we can use the present visual information from the scene to either recognize the kind ofplace it is (e.g., a kitchen or a bedroom) or navigate through it. Here we directly test the hypothesis that these two processes, what we call “scene categorization” and “visually-guided navigation”, are supported by dissociable neural systems. Specifically, we manipulated task demands by asking human participants (male and female) to perform a scene categorization, visually-guided navigation, and baseline task on images of scenes, and measured both the average univariate responses and multivariate spatial pattern of responses within two scene-selective cortical regions, the parahippocampal place area (PPA) and occipital place area (OPA), hypothesized to be separably involved in scene categorization and visually-guided navigation, respectively. As predicted, in the univariate analysis, PPA responded significantly more during the categorization task than during both the navigation and baseline tasks, whereas OPA showed the complete opposite pattern. Similarly, in the multivariate analysis, a linear support vector machine achieved above-chance classification for the categorization task, but not the navigation task in PPA. By contrast, above-chance classification was achieved for both the navigation and categorization tasks in OPA. However, above-chance classification for both tasks was also found in early visual cortex and hence not specific to OPA, suggesting that the spatial patterns of responses in OPA are merely inherited from early vision, and thus may be epiphenomenal to behavior. Together, these results are evidence for dissociable neural systems involved in recognizing places and navigating through them.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1200-18.2018

Close

Eliska Prochazkova; Luisa Prochazkova; Michael Rojek Giffin; H. Steven Scholte; Carsten K. W. De Dreu; Mariska E. Kret

Pupil mimicry promotes trust through the theory-of-mind network Journal Article

In: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 115, no. 31, pp. E7265–E7274, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Prochazkova2018,
title = {Pupil mimicry promotes trust through the theory-of-mind network},
author = {Eliska Prochazkova and Luisa Prochazkova and Michael Rojek Giffin and H. Steven Scholte and Carsten K. W. De Dreu and Mariska E. Kret},
doi = {10.1073/pnas.1803916115},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences},
volume = {115},
number = {31},
pages = {E7265--E7274},
abstract = {The human eye can provide powerful insights into the emotions and intentions of others; however, how pupillary changes influence observers' behavior remains largely unknown. The present fMRI–pupillometry study revealed that when the pupils of interacting partners synchronously dilate, trust is promoted, which suggests that pupil mimicry affiliates people. Here we provide evidence that pupil mimicry modulates trust decisions through the activation of the theory-of-mind network (precuneus, temporo-parietal junction, superior temporal sulcus, and medial prefrontal cortex). This network was recruited during pupil-dilation mimicry compared with interactions without mimicry or compared with pupil-constriction mimicry. Furthermore, the level of theory-of-mind engagement was proportional to individual's susceptibility to pupil-dilation mimicry. These data reveal a fundamental mechanism by which an individual's pupils trigger neurophysiological responses within an observer: when interacting partners synchronously dilate their pupils, humans come to feel reflections of the inner states of others, which fosters trust formation.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The human eye can provide powerful insights into the emotions and intentions of others; however, how pupillary changes influence observers' behavior remains largely unknown. The present fMRI–pupillometry study revealed that when the pupils of interacting partners synchronously dilate, trust is promoted, which suggests that pupil mimicry affiliates people. Here we provide evidence that pupil mimicry modulates trust decisions through the activation of the theory-of-mind network (precuneus, temporo-parietal junction, superior temporal sulcus, and medial prefrontal cortex). This network was recruited during pupil-dilation mimicry compared with interactions without mimicry or compared with pupil-constriction mimicry. Furthermore, the level of theory-of-mind engagement was proportional to individual's susceptibility to pupil-dilation mimicry. These data reveal a fundamental mechanism by which an individual's pupils trigger neurophysiological responses within an observer: when interacting partners synchronously dilate their pupils, humans come to feel reflections of the inner states of others, which fosters trust formation.

Close

  • doi:10.1073/pnas.1803916115

Close

Masih Rahmati; Golbarg T. Saber; Clayton E. Curtis

Population dynamics of early visual cortex during working memory Journal Article

In: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 219–233, 2018.

Abstract | BibTeX

@article{Rahmati2018,
title = {Population dynamics of early visual cortex during working memory},
author = {Masih Rahmati and Golbarg T. Saber and Clayton E. Curtis},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
volume = {30},
number = {2},
pages = {219--233},
abstract = {Although the content of working memory (WM) can be decoded from the spatial patterns of brain activity in early visual cortex, how populations encode WM representations remains unclear.Here, we address this limitation by using a model-based approach that reconstructs the feature encoded by population activity measured with fMRI. Using this approach, we could successfully reconstruct the locations of memory-guided saccade goals based on the pattern of activity in visual cortex during a memory delay. We could reconstruct the saccade goal even when we dissociated the visual stimulus from the saccade goal using a memory-guided antisaccade procedure. By comparing the spatiotemporal population dynamics, we find that the representations in visual cortex are stable but can also evolve from a representation of a remembered visual stimulus to a prospective goal. Moreover, because the representation of the antisaccade goal cannot be the result of bottom–up visual stimulation, it must be evoked by top–down signals presumably originating from frontal and/ or parietal cortex. Indeed, we find that trial-by-trial fluctuations in delay period activity in frontal and parietal cortex correlate with the precision with which our model reconstructed the maintained saccade goal based on the pattern of activity in visual cortex. Therefore, the population dynamics in visual cortex encode WM representations, and these representations can be sculpted by top–down signals from frontal and parietal cortex.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Although the content of working memory (WM) can be decoded from the spatial patterns of brain activity in early visual cortex, how populations encode WM representations remains unclear.Here, we address this limitation by using a model-based approach that reconstructs the feature encoded by population activity measured with fMRI. Using this approach, we could successfully reconstruct the locations of memory-guided saccade goals based on the pattern of activity in visual cortex during a memory delay. We could reconstruct the saccade goal even when we dissociated the visual stimulus from the saccade goal using a memory-guided antisaccade procedure. By comparing the spatiotemporal population dynamics, we find that the representations in visual cortex are stable but can also evolve from a representation of a remembered visual stimulus to a prospective goal. Moreover, because the representation of the antisaccade goal cannot be the result of bottom–up visual stimulation, it must be evoked by top–down signals presumably originating from frontal and/ or parietal cortex. Indeed, we find that trial-by-trial fluctuations in delay period activity in frontal and parietal cortex correlate with the precision with which our model reconstructed the maintained saccade goal based on the pattern of activity in visual cortex. Therefore, the population dynamics in visual cortex encode WM representations, and these representations can be sculpted by top–down signals from frontal and parietal cortex.

Close

Ignacio Rebollo; Anne-Dominique Devauchelle; Benoît Béranger; Catherine Tallon-Baudry

Stomach-brain synchrony reveals a novel, delayed-connectivity resting-state network in humans Journal Article

In: eLife, vol. 7, pp. 1–25, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Rebollo2018,
title = {Stomach-brain synchrony reveals a novel, delayed-connectivity resting-state network in humans},
author = {Ignacio Rebollo and Anne-Dominique Devauchelle and Benoît Béranger and Catherine Tallon-Baudry},
doi = {10.7554/elife.33321},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {eLife},
volume = {7},
pages = {1--25},
abstract = {Resting-state networks offer a unique window into the brain's functional architecture, but their characterization remains limited to instantaneous connectivity thus far. Here, we describe a novel resting-state network based on the delayed connectivity between the brain and the slow electrical rhythm (0.05 Hz) generated in the stomach. The gastric network cuts across classical resting-state networks with partial overlap with autonomic regulation areas. This network is composed of regions with convergent functional properties involved in mapping bodily space through touch, action or vision, as well as mapping external space in bodily coordinates. The network is characterized by a precise temporal sequence of activations within a gastric cycle, beginning with somato-motor cortices and ending with the extrastriate body area and dorsal precuneus. Our results demonstrate that canonical resting-state networks based on instantaneous connectivity represent only one of the possible partitions of the brain into coherent networks based on temporal dynamics.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Resting-state networks offer a unique window into the brain's functional architecture, but their characterization remains limited to instantaneous connectivity thus far. Here, we describe a novel resting-state network based on the delayed connectivity between the brain and the slow electrical rhythm (0.05 Hz) generated in the stomach. The gastric network cuts across classical resting-state networks with partial overlap with autonomic regulation areas. This network is composed of regions with convergent functional properties involved in mapping bodily space through touch, action or vision, as well as mapping external space in bodily coordinates. The network is characterized by a precise temporal sequence of activations within a gastric cycle, beginning with somato-motor cortices and ending with the extrastriate body area and dorsal precuneus. Our results demonstrate that canonical resting-state networks based on instantaneous connectivity represent only one of the possible partitions of the brain into coherent networks based on temporal dynamics.

Close

  • doi:10.7554/elife.33321

Close

Johannes Rennig; Michael S. Beauchamp

Free viewing of talking faces reveals mouth and eye preferring regions of the human superior temporal sulcus Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 183, pp. 25–36, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Rennig2018,
title = {Free viewing of talking faces reveals mouth and eye preferring regions of the human superior temporal sulcus},
author = {Johannes Rennig and Michael S. Beauchamp},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.008},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {183},
pages = {25--36},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {During face-to-face communication, the mouth of the talker is informative about speech content, while the eyes of the talker convey other information, such as gaze location. Viewers most often fixate either the mouth or the eyes of the talker's face, presumably allowing them to sample these different sources of information. To study the neural correlates of this process, healthy humans freely viewed talking faces while brain activity was measured with BOLD fMRI and eye movements were recorded with a video-based eye tracker. Post hoc trial sorting was used to divide the data into trials in which participants fixated the mouth of the talker and trials in which they fixated the eyes. Although the audiovisual stimulus was identical, the two trials types evoked differing responses in subregions of the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS). The anterior pSTS preferred trials in which participants fixated the mouth of the talker while the posterior pSTS preferred fixations on the eye of the talker. A second fMRI experiment demonstrated that anterior pSTS responded more strongly to auditory and audiovisual speech than posterior pSTS eye-preferring regions. These results provide evidence for functional specialization within the pSTS under more realistic viewing and stimulus conditions than in previous neuroimaging studies.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

During face-to-face communication, the mouth of the talker is informative about speech content, while the eyes of the talker convey other information, such as gaze location. Viewers most often fixate either the mouth or the eyes of the talker's face, presumably allowing them to sample these different sources of information. To study the neural correlates of this process, healthy humans freely viewed talking faces while brain activity was measured with BOLD fMRI and eye movements were recorded with a video-based eye tracker. Post hoc trial sorting was used to divide the data into trials in which participants fixated the mouth of the talker and trials in which they fixated the eyes. Although the audiovisual stimulus was identical, the two trials types evoked differing responses in subregions of the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS). The anterior pSTS preferred trials in which participants fixated the mouth of the talker while the posterior pSTS preferred fixations on the eye of the talker. A second fMRI experiment demonstrated that anterior pSTS responded more strongly to auditory and audiovisual speech than posterior pSTS eye-preferring regions. These results provide evidence for functional specialization within the pSTS under more realistic viewing and stimulus conditions than in previous neuroimaging studies.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.008

Close

Maya L. Rosen; Chantal E. Stern; Kathryn J. Devaney; David C. Somers

Cortical and subcortical contributions to long-term memory-guided visuospatial attention Journal Article

In: Cerebral Cortex, vol. 28, no. 8, pp. 2935–2947, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Rosen2018,
title = {Cortical and subcortical contributions to long-term memory-guided visuospatial attention},
author = {Maya L. Rosen and Chantal E. Stern and Kathryn J. Devaney and David C. Somers},
doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhx172},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
volume = {28},
number = {8},
pages = {2935--2947},
abstract = {Long-term memory (LTM) helps to efficiently direct and deploy the scarce resources of the attentional system; however, the neural substrates that support LTM-guidance of visual attention are not well understood. Here, we present results from fMRI experiments that demonstrate that cortical and subcortical regions of a network defined by resting-state functional connectivity are selectively recruited for LTM-guided attention, relative to a similarly demanding stimulus-guided attention paradigm that lacks memory retrieval and relative to a memory retrieval paradigm that lacks covert deployment of attention. Memory-guided visuospatial attention recruited posterior callosal sulcus, posterior precuneus, and lateral intraparietal sulcus bilaterally. Additionally, 3 subcortical regions defined by intrinsic functional connectivity were recruited: the caudate head, mediodorsal thalamus, and cerebellar lobule VI/Crus I. Although the broad resting-state network to which these nodes belong has been referred to as a cognitive control network, the posterior cortical regions activated in the present study are not typically identified with supporting standard cognitive control tasks. We propose that these regions form a Memory-Attention Network that is recruited for processes that integrate mnemonic and stimulus-based representations to guide attention. These findings may have important implications for understanding the mechanisms by which memory retrieval influences attentional deployment.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Long-term memory (LTM) helps to efficiently direct and deploy the scarce resources of the attentional system; however, the neural substrates that support LTM-guidance of visual attention are not well understood. Here, we present results from fMRI experiments that demonstrate that cortical and subcortical regions of a network defined by resting-state functional connectivity are selectively recruited for LTM-guided attention, relative to a similarly demanding stimulus-guided attention paradigm that lacks memory retrieval and relative to a memory retrieval paradigm that lacks covert deployment of attention. Memory-guided visuospatial attention recruited posterior callosal sulcus, posterior precuneus, and lateral intraparietal sulcus bilaterally. Additionally, 3 subcortical regions defined by intrinsic functional connectivity were recruited: the caudate head, mediodorsal thalamus, and cerebellar lobule VI/Crus I. Although the broad resting-state network to which these nodes belong has been referred to as a cognitive control network, the posterior cortical regions activated in the present study are not typically identified with supporting standard cognitive control tasks. We propose that these regions form a Memory-Attention Network that is recruited for processes that integrate mnemonic and stimulus-based representations to guide attention. These findings may have important implications for understanding the mechanisms by which memory retrieval influences attentional deployment.

Close

  • doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx172

Close

Ricky R. Savjani; Sucharit Katyal; Elizabeth Halfen; Jung Hwan Kim; David Ress

Polar-angle representation of saccadic eye movements in human superior colliculus Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 171, pp. 199–208, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Savjani2018,
title = {Polar-angle representation of saccadic eye movements in human superior colliculus},
author = {Ricky R. Savjani and Sucharit Katyal and Elizabeth Halfen and Jung Hwan Kim and David Ress},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.080},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {171},
pages = {199--208},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {The superior colliculus (SC) is a layered midbrain structure involved in directing both head and eye movements and coordinating visual attention. Although a retinotopic organization for the mediation of saccadic eye-movements has been shown in monkey SC, in human SC the topography of saccades has not been confirmed. Here, a novel experimental paradigm was performed by five participants (one female) while high-resolution (1.2-mm) functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to measure activity evoked by saccadic eye movements within human SC. Results provide three critical observations about the topography of the SC: (1) saccades along the superior-inferior visual axis are mapped across the medial-lateral anatomy of the SC; (2) the saccadic eye-movement representation is in register with the retinotopic organization of visual stimulation; and (3) activity evoked by saccades occurs deeper within SC than that evoked by visual stimulation. These approaches lay the foundation for studying the organization of human subcortical – and enhanced cortical mapping – of eye-movement mechanisms.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The superior colliculus (SC) is a layered midbrain structure involved in directing both head and eye movements and coordinating visual attention. Although a retinotopic organization for the mediation of saccadic eye-movements has been shown in monkey SC, in human SC the topography of saccades has not been confirmed. Here, a novel experimental paradigm was performed by five participants (one female) while high-resolution (1.2-mm) functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to measure activity evoked by saccadic eye movements within human SC. Results provide three critical observations about the topography of the SC: (1) saccades along the superior-inferior visual axis are mapped across the medial-lateral anatomy of the SC; (2) the saccadic eye-movement representation is in register with the retinotopic organization of visual stimulation; and (3) activity evoked by saccades occurs deeper within SC than that evoked by visual stimulation. These approaches lay the foundation for studying the organization of human subcortical – and enhanced cortical mapping – of eye-movement mechanisms.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.080

Close

Max Schneider; Laura Leuchs; Michael Czisch; Philipp G. Sämann; Victor I. Spoormaker

Disentangling reward anticipation with simultaneous pupillometry / fMRI Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 178, pp. 11–22, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Schneider2018,
title = {Disentangling reward anticipation with simultaneous pupillometry / fMRI},
author = {Max Schneider and Laura Leuchs and Michael Czisch and Philipp G. Sämann and Victor I. Spoormaker},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.078},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {178},
pages = {11--22},
abstract = {The reward system may provide an interesting intermediate phenotype for anhedonia in affective disorders. Reward anticipation is characterized by an increase in arousal, and previous studies have linked the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) to arousal responses such as dilation of the pupil. Here, we examined pupil dynamics during a reward anticipation task in forty-six healthy human subjects and evaluated its neural correlates using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Pupil size showed a strong increase during monetary reward anticipation, a moderate increase during verbal reward anticipation and a decrease during control trials. For fMRI analyses, average pupil size and pupil change were computed in 1-s time bins during the anticipation phase. Activity in the ventral striatum was inversely related to the pupil size time course, indicating an early onset of activation and a role in reward prediction processing. Pupil dilations were linked to increased activity in the salience network (dorsal ACC and bilateral insula), which likely triggers an increase in arousal to enhance task performance. Finally, increased pupil size preceding the required motor response was associated with activity in the ventral attention network. In sum, pupillometry provides an effective tool for disentangling different phases of reward anticipation, with relevance for affective symptomatology.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The reward system may provide an interesting intermediate phenotype for anhedonia in affective disorders. Reward anticipation is characterized by an increase in arousal, and previous studies have linked the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) to arousal responses such as dilation of the pupil. Here, we examined pupil dynamics during a reward anticipation task in forty-six healthy human subjects and evaluated its neural correlates using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Pupil size showed a strong increase during monetary reward anticipation, a moderate increase during verbal reward anticipation and a decrease during control trials. For fMRI analyses, average pupil size and pupil change were computed in 1-s time bins during the anticipation phase. Activity in the ventral striatum was inversely related to the pupil size time course, indicating an early onset of activation and a role in reward prediction processing. Pupil dilations were linked to increased activity in the salience network (dorsal ACC and bilateral insula), which likely triggers an increase in arousal to enhance task performance. Finally, increased pupil size preceding the required motor response was associated with activity in the ventral attention network. In sum, pupillometry provides an effective tool for disentangling different phases of reward anticipation, with relevance for affective symptomatology.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.078

Close

Oleg Solopchuk; Moustapha Sebti; Céline Bouvy; Charles-Etienne Benoit; Thibault Warlop; Anne Jeanjean; Alexandre Zénon

Locus Coeruleus atrophy doesn't relate to fatigue in Parkinson's disease Journal Article

In: Scientific Reports, vol. 8, pp. 12381, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Solopchuk2018,
title = {Locus Coeruleus atrophy doesn't relate to fatigue in Parkinson's disease},
author = {Oleg Solopchuk and Moustapha Sebti and Céline Bouvy and Charles-Etienne Benoit and Thibault Warlop and Anne Jeanjean and Alexandre Zénon},
doi = {10.1038/s41598-018-30128-y},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {8},
pages = {12381},
abstract = {Fatigue is a frequent complaint among healthy population and one of the earliest and most debilitating symptoms in Parkinson's disease (PD). Earlier studies have examined the role of dopamine and serotonin in pathogenesis of fatigue, but the plausible role of noradrenalin (NA) remains underexplored. We investigated the relationship between fatigue in Parkinsonian patients and the extent of degeneration of Locus Coeruleus (LC), the main source of NA in the brain. We quantified LC and Substantia Nigra (SN) atrophy using neuromelanin-sensitive imaging, analyzed with a novel, fully automated algorithm. We also assessed patients' fatigue, depression, sleep disturbance and vigilance. We found that LC degeneration correlated with the levels of depression and vigilance but not with fatigue, while fatigue correlated weakly with atrophy of SN. These results indicate that LC degeneration in Parkinson's disease is unlikely to cause fatigue, but may be involved in mood and vigilance alterations.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Fatigue is a frequent complaint among healthy population and one of the earliest and most debilitating symptoms in Parkinson's disease (PD). Earlier studies have examined the role of dopamine and serotonin in pathogenesis of fatigue, but the plausible role of noradrenalin (NA) remains underexplored. We investigated the relationship between fatigue in Parkinsonian patients and the extent of degeneration of Locus Coeruleus (LC), the main source of NA in the brain. We quantified LC and Substantia Nigra (SN) atrophy using neuromelanin-sensitive imaging, analyzed with a novel, fully automated algorithm. We also assessed patients' fatigue, depression, sleep disturbance and vigilance. We found that LC degeneration correlated with the levels of depression and vigilance but not with fatigue, while fatigue correlated weakly with atrophy of SN. These results indicate that LC degeneration in Parkinson's disease is unlikely to cause fatigue, but may be involved in mood and vigilance alterations.

Close

  • doi:10.1038/s41598-018-30128-y

Close

Chen Song; Geraint Rees

Intra-hemispheric integration underlies perception of tilt illusion Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 175, pp. 80–90, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Song2018,
title = {Intra-hemispheric integration underlies perception of tilt illusion},
author = {Chen Song and Geraint Rees},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.073},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {175},
pages = {80--90},
publisher = {The Author(s)},
abstract = {The integration of inputs across the entire visual field into a single conscious experience is fundamental to human visual perception. This integrated nature of visual experience is illustrated by contextual illusions such as the tilt illusion, in which the perceived orientation of a central grating appears tilted away from its physical orientation, due to the modulation by a surrounding grating with a different orientation. Here we investigated the relative contribution of local, intra-hemispheric and global, inter-hemispheric integration mechanisms to perception of the tilt illusion. We used Dynamic Causal Modelling of fMRI signals to estimate effective connectivity in human early visual cortices (V1, V2, V3) during bilateral presentation of a tilt illusion stimulus. Our analysis revealed that neural responses associated with the tilt illusion were modulated by intra- rather than inter-hemispheric connectivity. Crucially, across participants, intra-hemispheric connectivity in V1 correlated with the magnitude of the tilt illusion, while no such correlation was observed for V1 inter-hemispheric connectivity, or V2, V3 connectivity. Moreover, when the illusion stimulus was presented unilaterally rather than bilaterally, the illusion magnitude did not change. Together our findings suggest that perception of the tilt illusion reflects an intra-hemispheric integration mechanism. This is in contrast to the existing literature, which suggests inter-hemispheric modulation of neural activity as early as V1. This discrepancy with our findings may reflect the diversity and complexity of integration mechanisms involved in visual processing and visual perception.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The integration of inputs across the entire visual field into a single conscious experience is fundamental to human visual perception. This integrated nature of visual experience is illustrated by contextual illusions such as the tilt illusion, in which the perceived orientation of a central grating appears tilted away from its physical orientation, due to the modulation by a surrounding grating with a different orientation. Here we investigated the relative contribution of local, intra-hemispheric and global, inter-hemispheric integration mechanisms to perception of the tilt illusion. We used Dynamic Causal Modelling of fMRI signals to estimate effective connectivity in human early visual cortices (V1, V2, V3) during bilateral presentation of a tilt illusion stimulus. Our analysis revealed that neural responses associated with the tilt illusion were modulated by intra- rather than inter-hemispheric connectivity. Crucially, across participants, intra-hemispheric connectivity in V1 correlated with the magnitude of the tilt illusion, while no such correlation was observed for V1 inter-hemispheric connectivity, or V2, V3 connectivity. Moreover, when the illusion stimulus was presented unilaterally rather than bilaterally, the illusion magnitude did not change. Together our findings suggest that perception of the tilt illusion reflects an intra-hemispheric integration mechanism. This is in contrast to the existing literature, which suggests inter-hemispheric modulation of neural activity as early as V1. This discrepancy with our findings may reflect the diversity and complexity of integration mechanisms involved in visual processing and visual perception.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.073

Close

Teresa Sousa; Alexandre Sayal; João V. Duarte; Gabriel N. Costa; Ricardo Martins; Miguel Castelo-Branco

Evidence for distinct levels of neural adaptation to both coherent and incoherently moving visual surfaces in visual area hMT+ Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 179, pp. 540–547, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Sousa2018,
title = {Evidence for distinct levels of neural adaptation to both coherent and incoherently moving visual surfaces in visual area hMT+},
author = {Teresa Sousa and Alexandre Sayal and João V. Duarte and Gabriel N. Costa and Ricardo Martins and Miguel Castelo-Branco},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.075},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {179},
pages = {540--547},
abstract = {Visual adaptation describes the processes by which the visual system alters its operating properties in response to changes in the environment. It is one of the mechanisms controlling visual perceptual bistability – when two perceptual solutions are available – by controlling the duration of each percept. Moving plaids are an example of such ambiguity. They can be perceived as two surfaces sliding incoherently over each other or as a single coherent surface. Here, we investigated, using fMRI, whether activity in the human motion complex (hMT+), a region tightly related to the perceptual integration of visual motion, is modulated by distinct forms of visual adaptation to coherent or incoherent perception of moving plaids. Our hypothesis is that exposure to global coherent or incoherent moving stimuli leads to different levels of measurable adaptation, reflected in hMT+ activity. We found that the strength of the measured visual adaptation effect depended on whether subjects integrated (coherent percept) or segregated (incoherent percept) surface motion signals. Visual motion adaptation was significant both for coherent motion and globally incoherent surface motion. Although not as strong as to the coherent percept, visual adaptation due to the incoherent percept also affects hMT+. This shows that adaptation can contribute to regulate percept duration during visual bistability, with distinct weights, depending on the type of percept. Our findings suggest a link between bistability and adaptation mechanisms, both due to coherent and incoherent motion percepts, but in an asymmetric manner. These asymmetric adaptation weights have strong implications in models of perceptual decision and may explain asymmetry of perceptual interpretation periods.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Visual adaptation describes the processes by which the visual system alters its operating properties in response to changes in the environment. It is one of the mechanisms controlling visual perceptual bistability – when two perceptual solutions are available – by controlling the duration of each percept. Moving plaids are an example of such ambiguity. They can be perceived as two surfaces sliding incoherently over each other or as a single coherent surface. Here, we investigated, using fMRI, whether activity in the human motion complex (hMT+), a region tightly related to the perceptual integration of visual motion, is modulated by distinct forms of visual adaptation to coherent or incoherent perception of moving plaids. Our hypothesis is that exposure to global coherent or incoherent moving stimuli leads to different levels of measurable adaptation, reflected in hMT+ activity. We found that the strength of the measured visual adaptation effect depended on whether subjects integrated (coherent percept) or segregated (incoherent percept) surface motion signals. Visual motion adaptation was significant both for coherent motion and globally incoherent surface motion. Although not as strong as to the coherent percept, visual adaptation due to the incoherent percept also affects hMT+. This shows that adaptation can contribute to regulate percept duration during visual bistability, with distinct weights, depending on the type of percept. Our findings suggest a link between bistability and adaptation mechanisms, both due to coherent and incoherent motion percepts, but in an asymmetric manner. These asymmetric adaptation weights have strong implications in models of perceptual decision and may explain asymmetry of perceptual interpretation periods.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.075

Close

Maria Steffens; C. Neumann; Anna-Maria Kasparbauer; B. Becker; Bernd Weber; Mitul A. Mehta; R. Hurlemann; Ulrich Ettinger

Effects of ketamine on brain function during response inhibition Journal Article

In: Psychopharmacology, vol. 235, no. 12, pp. 3559–3571, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Steffens2018,
title = {Effects of ketamine on brain function during response inhibition},
author = {Maria Steffens and C. Neumann and Anna-Maria Kasparbauer and B. Becker and Bernd Weber and Mitul A. Mehta and R. Hurlemann and Ulrich Ettinger},
doi = {10.1007/s00213-018-5081-7},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
urldate = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Psychopharmacology},
volume = {235},
number = {12},
pages = {3559--3571},
publisher = {Psychopharmacology},
abstract = {Introduction The uncompetitive N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor (NMDAR) antagonist ketamine has been proposed to model symptoms ofpsychosis. Inhibitory deficits in the schizophrenia spectrumhave been reliably reported using the antisaccade task. Interestingly, although similar antisaccade deficits have been reported following ketamine in non-human primates, ketamine-induced deficits have not been observed in healthy human volunteers. Methods To investigate the effects of ketamine on brain function during an antisaccade task, we conducted a double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subjects study on n = 15 healthy males. We measured the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) response and eye movements during a mixed antisaccade/prosaccade task while participants received a subanesthetic dose of intravenous ketamine (target plasma level 100 ng/ml) on one occasion and placebo on the other occasion. Results While ketamine significantly increased self-ratings of psychosis-like experiences, it did not induce antisaccade or prosaccade performance deficits. At the level of BOLD, we observed an interaction between treatment and task condition in somatosensory cortex, suggesting recruitment of additional neural resources in the antisaccade condition under NMDAR blockage. Discussion Given the robust evidence ofantisaccade deficits in schizophrenia spectrum populations, the current findings suggest that ketamine may not mimic all features ofpsychosis at the dose used in this study. Our findings underline the importance of a more detailed research to further understand and define effects of NMDAR hypofunction on human brain function and behavior, with a view to applying ketamine administration as a model system of psychosis. Future studies with varying doses will be of importance in this context.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Introduction The uncompetitive N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor (NMDAR) antagonist ketamine has been proposed to model symptoms ofpsychosis. Inhibitory deficits in the schizophrenia spectrumhave been reliably reported using the antisaccade task. Interestingly, although similar antisaccade deficits have been reported following ketamine in non-human primates, ketamine-induced deficits have not been observed in healthy human volunteers. Methods To investigate the effects of ketamine on brain function during an antisaccade task, we conducted a double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subjects study on n = 15 healthy males. We measured the blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) response and eye movements during a mixed antisaccade/prosaccade task while participants received a subanesthetic dose of intravenous ketamine (target plasma level 100 ng/ml) on one occasion and placebo on the other occasion. Results While ketamine significantly increased self-ratings of psychosis-like experiences, it did not induce antisaccade or prosaccade performance deficits. At the level of BOLD, we observed an interaction between treatment and task condition in somatosensory cortex, suggesting recruitment of additional neural resources in the antisaccade condition under NMDAR blockage. Discussion Given the robust evidence ofantisaccade deficits in schizophrenia spectrum populations, the current findings suggest that ketamine may not mimic all features ofpsychosis at the dose used in this study. Our findings underline the importance of a more detailed research to further understand and define effects of NMDAR hypofunction on human brain function and behavior, with a view to applying ketamine administration as a model system of psychosis. Future studies with varying doses will be of importance in this context.

Close

  • doi:10.1007/s00213-018-5081-7

Close

Daniel Marten Es; Jan Theeuwes; Tomas Knapen

Spatial sampling in human visual cortex is modulated by both spatial and feature-based attention Journal Article

In: eLife, vol. 7, pp. 1–28, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Es2018,
title = {Spatial sampling in human visual cortex is modulated by both spatial and feature-based attention},
author = {Daniel Marten Es and Jan Theeuwes and Tomas Knapen},
doi = {10.7554/elife.36928},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {eLife},
volume = {7},
pages = {1--28},
abstract = {Spatial attention changes the sampling of visual space. Behavioral studies suggest that feature-based attention modulates this resampling to optimize the attended feature's sampling. We investigate this hypothesis by estimating spatial sampling in visual cortex while independently varying both feature-based and spatial attention. Our results show that spatial and feature-based attention interacted: resampling of visual space depended on both the attended location and feature (color vs. temporal frequency). This interaction occurred similarly throughout visual cortex, regardless of an area's overall feature preference. However, the interaction did depend on spatial sampling properties of voxels that prefer the attended feature. These findings are parsimoniously explained by variations in the precision of an attentional gain field. Our results demonstrate that the deployment of spatial attention is tailored to the spatial sampling properties of units that are sensitive to the attended feature.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Spatial attention changes the sampling of visual space. Behavioral studies suggest that feature-based attention modulates this resampling to optimize the attended feature's sampling. We investigate this hypothesis by estimating spatial sampling in visual cortex while independently varying both feature-based and spatial attention. Our results show that spatial and feature-based attention interacted: resampling of visual space depended on both the attended location and feature (color vs. temporal frequency). This interaction occurred similarly throughout visual cortex, regardless of an area's overall feature preference. However, the interaction did depend on spatial sampling properties of voxels that prefer the attended feature. These findings are parsimoniously explained by variations in the precision of an attentional gain field. Our results demonstrate that the deployment of spatial attention is tailored to the spatial sampling properties of units that are sensitive to the attended feature.

Close

  • doi:10.7554/elife.36928

Close

Koen Lith; Dick Johan Veltman; Moran Daniel Cohn; Louise Else Pape; Marieke Eleonora Akker-Nijdam; Amanda Wilhelmina Geertruida Loon; Pierre Bet; Guido Alexander Wingen; Wim Brink; Theo Doreleijers; Arne Popma

Effects of methylphenidate during fear learning in antisocial adolescents: A randomized controlled fMRI trial Journal Article

In: Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 57, no. 12, pp. 934–943, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Lith2018,
title = {Effects of methylphenidate during fear learning in antisocial adolescents: A randomized controlled fMRI trial},
author = {Koen Lith and Dick Johan Veltman and Moran Daniel Cohn and Louise Else Pape and Marieke Eleonora Akker-Nijdam and Amanda Wilhelmina Geertruida Loon and Pierre Bet and Guido Alexander Wingen and Wim Brink and Theo Doreleijers and Arne Popma},
doi = {10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.026},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry},
volume = {57},
number = {12},
pages = {934--943},
publisher = {Elsevier Inc},
abstract = {Objective: Although the neural underpinnings of antisocial behavior have been studied extensively, research on pharmacologic interventions targeting specific neural mechanisms remains sparse. Hypoactivity of the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) has been reported in antisocial adolescents, which could account for deficits in fear learning (amygdala) and impairments in decision making (vmPFC), respectively. Limited clinical research suggests positive effects of methylphenidate, a dopamine agonist, on antisocial behavior in adolescents. Dopamine is a key neurotransmitter involved in amygdala and vmPFC functioning. The objective of this study was to investigate whether methylphenidate targets dysfunctions in these brain areas in adolescents with antisocial behavior. Method: A group of 42 clinical referred male adolescents (14–17 years old) with a disruptive behavior disorder performed a fear learning/reversal paradigm in a randomized double-blinded placebo-controlled pharmacologic functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Participants with disruptive behavior disorder were randomized to receive a single dose of methylphenidate 0.3 to 0.4 mg/kg (n = 22) or placebo (n = 20) and were compared with 21 matched healthy controls not receiving medication. Results: In a region-of-interest analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data during fear learning, the placebo group showed hyporeactivity of the amygdala compared with healthy controls, whereas amygdala reactivity was normalized in the methylphenidate group. There were no group differences in vmPFC reactivity during fear reversal learning. Whole-brain analyses showed no group differences. Conclusion: These findings suggest that methylphenidate is a promising pharmacologic intervention for youth antisocial behavior that could restore amygdala functioning.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Objective: Although the neural underpinnings of antisocial behavior have been studied extensively, research on pharmacologic interventions targeting specific neural mechanisms remains sparse. Hypoactivity of the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) has been reported in antisocial adolescents, which could account for deficits in fear learning (amygdala) and impairments in decision making (vmPFC), respectively. Limited clinical research suggests positive effects of methylphenidate, a dopamine agonist, on antisocial behavior in adolescents. Dopamine is a key neurotransmitter involved in amygdala and vmPFC functioning. The objective of this study was to investigate whether methylphenidate targets dysfunctions in these brain areas in adolescents with antisocial behavior. Method: A group of 42 clinical referred male adolescents (14–17 years old) with a disruptive behavior disorder performed a fear learning/reversal paradigm in a randomized double-blinded placebo-controlled pharmacologic functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Participants with disruptive behavior disorder were randomized to receive a single dose of methylphenidate 0.3 to 0.4 mg/kg (n = 22) or placebo (n = 20) and were compared with 21 matched healthy controls not receiving medication. Results: In a region-of-interest analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data during fear learning, the placebo group showed hyporeactivity of the amygdala compared with healthy controls, whereas amygdala reactivity was normalized in the methylphenidate group. There were no group differences in vmPFC reactivity during fear reversal learning. Whole-brain analyses showed no group differences. Conclusion: These findings suggest that methylphenidate is a promising pharmacologic intervention for youth antisocial behavior that could restore amygdala functioning.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.026

Close

Anouk Mariette Loon; Katya Olmos-Solis; Johannes J. Fahrenfort; Christian N. L. Olivers

Current and future goals are represented in opposite patterns in object-selective cortex Journal Article

In: eLife, vol. 7, pp. 1–25, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Loon2018,
title = {Current and future goals are represented in opposite patterns in object-selective cortex},
author = {Anouk Mariette Loon and Katya Olmos-Solis and Johannes J. Fahrenfort and Christian N. L. Olivers},
doi = {10.7554/elife.38677},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {eLife},
volume = {7},
pages = {1--25},
abstract = {Adaptive behavior requires the separation of current from future goals in working memory. We used fMRI of object-selective cortex to determine the representational (dis)similarities of memory representations serving current and prospective perceptual tasks. Participants remembered an object drawn from three possible categories as the target for one of two consecutive visual search tasks. A cue indicated whether the target object should be looked for first (currently relevant), second (prospectively relevant), or if it could be forgotten (irrelevant). Prior to the first search, representations of current, prospective and irrelevant objects were similar, with strongest decoding for current representations compared to prospective (Experiment 1) and irrelevant (Experiment 2). Remarkably, during the first search, prospective representations could also be decoded, but revealed anti-correlated voxel patterns compared to currently relevant representations of the same category. We propose that the brain separates current from prospective memories within the same neuronal ensembles through opposite representational patterns.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Adaptive behavior requires the separation of current from future goals in working memory. We used fMRI of object-selective cortex to determine the representational (dis)similarities of memory representations serving current and prospective perceptual tasks. Participants remembered an object drawn from three possible categories as the target for one of two consecutive visual search tasks. A cue indicated whether the target object should be looked for first (currently relevant), second (prospectively relevant), or if it could be forgotten (irrelevant). Prior to the first search, representations of current, prospective and irrelevant objects were similar, with strongest decoding for current representations compared to prospective (Experiment 1) and irrelevant (Experiment 2). Remarkably, during the first search, prospective representations could also be decoded, but revealed anti-correlated voxel patterns compared to currently relevant representations of the same category. We propose that the brain separates current from prospective memories within the same neuronal ensembles through opposite representational patterns.

Close

  • doi:10.7554/elife.38677

Close

Maryam Vaziri-Pashkam; JohnMark Taylor; Yaoda Xu

Spatial frequency tolerant visual object representations in the human ventral and dorsal visual processing pathways Journal Article

In: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 31, no. 1, pp. 49–63, 2018.

Abstract | BibTeX

@article{VaziriPashkam2018,
title = {Spatial frequency tolerant visual object representations in the human ventral and dorsal visual processing pathways},
author = {Maryam Vaziri-Pashkam and JohnMark Taylor and Yaoda Xu},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
volume = {31},
number = {1},
pages = {49--63},
abstract = {Primate ventral and dorsal visual pathways both contain visual object representations. Dorsal regions receive more input from magnocellular system while ventral regions receive inputs from both magnocellular and parvocellular systems. Due to potential differences in the spatial sensitivites of man- ocellular and parvocellular systems, object representations in ventral and dorsal regions may differ in how they represent visual input from different spatial scales. To test this prediction, we asked observers to view blocks of images from six object catego- ries, shown in full spectrum, high spatial frequency (SF), or low SF. We found robust object category decoding in all SF conditions as well as SF decoding in nearly all the early visual, ventral, and dorsal regions examined. Cross-SF decoding further revealed that object category representations in all regions exhibited sub- stantial tolerance across the SF components. No difference between ventral and dorsal regions was found in their preference for the different SF components. Further comparisons revealed that, whereas differences in the SF component separated object category representations in early visual areas, such a separation was much smaller in downstream ventral and dorsal regions. In those regions, variations among the object categories played a more significant role in shaping the visual representational structures. Our findings show that ventral and dorsal regions are sim- ilar in how they represent visual input from different spatial scales and argue against a dissociation of these regions based on differential sensitivity to different SFs.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Primate ventral and dorsal visual pathways both contain visual object representations. Dorsal regions receive more input from magnocellular system while ventral regions receive inputs from both magnocellular and parvocellular systems. Due to potential differences in the spatial sensitivites of man- ocellular and parvocellular systems, object representations in ventral and dorsal regions may differ in how they represent visual input from different spatial scales. To test this prediction, we asked observers to view blocks of images from six object catego- ries, shown in full spectrum, high spatial frequency (SF), or low SF. We found robust object category decoding in all SF conditions as well as SF decoding in nearly all the early visual, ventral, and dorsal regions examined. Cross-SF decoding further revealed that object category representations in all regions exhibited sub- stantial tolerance across the SF components. No difference between ventral and dorsal regions was found in their preference for the different SF components. Further comparisons revealed that, whereas differences in the SF component separated object category representations in early visual areas, such a separation was much smaller in downstream ventral and dorsal regions. In those regions, variations among the object categories played a more significant role in shaping the visual representational structures. Our findings show that ventral and dorsal regions are sim- ilar in how they represent visual input from different spatial scales and argue against a dissociation of these regions based on differential sensitivity to different SFs.

Close

Alain Guillaume; Jason R. Fuller; Riju Srimal; Clayton E. Curtis

Cortico-cerebellar network involved in saccade adaptation Journal Article

In: Journal of Neurophysiology, vol. 120, no. 5, pp. 2583–2594, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Guillaume2018,
title = {Cortico-cerebellar network involved in saccade adaptation},
author = {Alain Guillaume and Jason R. Fuller and Riju Srimal and Clayton E. Curtis},
doi = {10.1152/jn.00392.2018},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neurophysiology},
volume = {120},
number = {5},
pages = {2583--2594},
abstract = {Saccade adaptation is the learning process that en- sures that vision and saccades remain calibrated. The central nervous system network involved in these adaptive processes remains unclear because of difficulties in isolating the learning process from the correlated visual and motor processes. Here we imaged the human brain during a novel saccade adaptation paradigm that allowed us to isolate neural signals involved in learning independent of the changes in the amplitude of corrective saccades usually correlated with adap- tation. We show that the changes in activation in the ipsiversive cerebellar vermis that track adaptation are not driven by the changes in corrective saccades and thus provide critical supporting evidence for previous findings. Similarly, we find that activation in the dorso- medial wall of the contraversive precuneus mirrors the pattern found in the cerebellum. Finally, we identify dorsolateral and dorsomedial cortical areas in the frontal and parietal lobes that encode the retinal errors following inaccurate saccades used to drive recalibration. To- gether, these data identify a distributed network of cerebellar and cortical areas and their specific roles in oculomotor learning.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Saccade adaptation is the learning process that en- sures that vision and saccades remain calibrated. The central nervous system network involved in these adaptive processes remains unclear because of difficulties in isolating the learning process from the correlated visual and motor processes. Here we imaged the human brain during a novel saccade adaptation paradigm that allowed us to isolate neural signals involved in learning independent of the changes in the amplitude of corrective saccades usually correlated with adap- tation. We show that the changes in activation in the ipsiversive cerebellar vermis that track adaptation are not driven by the changes in corrective saccades and thus provide critical supporting evidence for previous findings. Similarly, we find that activation in the dorso- medial wall of the contraversive precuneus mirrors the pattern found in the cerebellum. Finally, we identify dorsolateral and dorsomedial cortical areas in the frontal and parietal lobes that encode the retinal errors following inaccurate saccades used to drive recalibration. To- gether, these data identify a distributed network of cerebellar and cortical areas and their specific roles in oculomotor learning.

Close

  • doi:10.1152/jn.00392.2018

Close

Michelle G. Hall; Claire K. Naughtin; Jason B. Mattingley; Paul E. Dux

Distributed and opposing effects of incidental learning in the human brain Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 173, pp. 351–360, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Hall2018,
title = {Distributed and opposing effects of incidental learning in the human brain},
author = {Michelle G. Hall and Claire K. Naughtin and Jason B. Mattingley and Paul E. Dux},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.068},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {173},
pages = {351--360},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {Incidental learning affords a behavioural advantage when sensory information matches regularities that have previously been encountered. Previous studies have taken a focused approach by probing the involvement of specific candidate brain regions underlying incidentally acquired memory representations, as well as expectation effects on early sensory representations. Here, we investigated the broader extent of the brain's sensitivity to violations and fulfilments of expectations, using an incidental learning paradigm in which the contingencies between target locations and target identities were manipulated without participants' overt knowledge. Multivariate analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data was applied to compare the consistency of neural activity for visual events that the contingency manipulation rendered likely versus unlikely. We observed widespread sensitivity to expectations across frontal, temporal, occipital, and sub-cortical areas. These activation clusters showed distinct response profiles, such that some regions displayed more reliable activation patterns under fulfilled expectations, whereas others showed more reliable patterns when expectations were violated. These findings reveal that expectations affect multiple stages of information processing during visual decision making, rather than early sensory processing stages alone.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Incidental learning affords a behavioural advantage when sensory information matches regularities that have previously been encountered. Previous studies have taken a focused approach by probing the involvement of specific candidate brain regions underlying incidentally acquired memory representations, as well as expectation effects on early sensory representations. Here, we investigated the broader extent of the brain's sensitivity to violations and fulfilments of expectations, using an incidental learning paradigm in which the contingencies between target locations and target identities were manipulated without participants' overt knowledge. Multivariate analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data was applied to compare the consistency of neural activity for visual events that the contingency manipulation rendered likely versus unlikely. We observed widespread sensitivity to expectations across frontal, temporal, occipital, and sub-cortical areas. These activation clusters showed distinct response profiles, such that some regions displayed more reliable activation patterns under fulfilled expectations, whereas others showed more reliable patterns when expectations were violated. These findings reveal that expectations affect multiple stages of information processing during visual decision making, rather than early sensory processing stages alone.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.02.068

Close

Michael P. Harms; Leah H. Somerville; Beau M. Ances; Jesper Andersson; Deanna M. Barch; Matteo Bastiani; Susan Y. Bookheimer; Timothy B. Brown; Randy L. Buckner; Gregory C. Burgess; Timothy S. Coalson; Michael A. Chappell; Mirella Dapretto; Gwenaëlle Douaud; Bruce Fischl; Matthew F. Glasser; Douglas N. Greve; Cynthia Hodge; Keith W. Jamison; Saad Jbabdi; Sridhar Kandala; Xiufeng Li; Ross W. Mair; Silvia Mangia; Daniel Marcus; Daniele Mascali; Steen Moeller; Thomas E. Nichols; Emma C. Robinson; David H. Salat; Stephen M. Smith; Stamatios N. Sotiropoulos; Melissa Terpstra; Kathleen M. Thomas; M. Dylan Tisdall; Kamil Ugurbil; Andre Kouwe; Roger P. Woods; Lilla Zöllei; David C. Van Essen; Essa Yacoub

Extending the Human Connectome Project across ages: Imaging protocols for the Lifespan Development and Aging projects Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 183, pp. 972–984, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Harms2018,
title = {Extending the Human Connectome Project across ages: Imaging protocols for the Lifespan Development and Aging projects},
author = {Michael P. Harms and Leah H. Somerville and Beau M. Ances and Jesper Andersson and Deanna M. Barch and Matteo Bastiani and Susan Y. Bookheimer and Timothy B. Brown and Randy L. Buckner and Gregory C. Burgess and Timothy S. Coalson and Michael A. Chappell and Mirella Dapretto and Gwenaëlle Douaud and Bruce Fischl and Matthew F. Glasser and Douglas N. Greve and Cynthia Hodge and Keith W. Jamison and Saad Jbabdi and Sridhar Kandala and Xiufeng Li and Ross W. Mair and Silvia Mangia and Daniel Marcus and Daniele Mascali and Steen Moeller and Thomas E. Nichols and Emma C. Robinson and David H. Salat and Stephen M. Smith and Stamatios N. Sotiropoulos and Melissa Terpstra and Kathleen M. Thomas and M. Dylan Tisdall and Kamil Ugurbil and Andre Kouwe and Roger P. Woods and Lilla Zöllei and David C. Van Essen and Essa Yacoub},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.060},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {183},
pages = {972--984},
abstract = {The Human Connectome Projects in Development (HCP-D) and Aging (HCP-A) are two large-scale brain imaging studies that will extend the recently completed HCP Young-Adult (HCP-YA) project to nearly the full lifespan, collecting structural, resting-state fMRI, task-fMRI, diffusion, and perfusion MRI in participants from 5 to 100+ years of age. HCP-D is enrolling 1300+ healthy children, adolescents, and young adults (ages 5–21), and HCP-A is enrolling 1200+ healthy adults (ages 36–100+), with each study collecting longitudinal data in a subset of individuals at particular age ranges. The imaging protocols of the HCP-D and HCP-A studies are very similar, differing primarily in the selection of different task-fMRI paradigms. We strove to harmonize the imaging protocol to the greatest extent feasible with the completed HCP-YA (1200+ participants, aged 22–35), but some imaging-related changes were motivated or necessitated by hardware changes, the need to reduce the total amount of scanning per participant, and/or the additional challenges of working with young and elderly populations. Here, we provide an overview of the common HCP-D/A imaging protocol including data and rationales for protocol decisions and changes relative to HCP-YA. The result will be a large, rich, multi-modal, and freely available set of consistently acquired data for use by the scientific community to investigate and define normative developmental and aging related changes in the healthy human brain.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The Human Connectome Projects in Development (HCP-D) and Aging (HCP-A) are two large-scale brain imaging studies that will extend the recently completed HCP Young-Adult (HCP-YA) project to nearly the full lifespan, collecting structural, resting-state fMRI, task-fMRI, diffusion, and perfusion MRI in participants from 5 to 100+ years of age. HCP-D is enrolling 1300+ healthy children, adolescents, and young adults (ages 5–21), and HCP-A is enrolling 1200+ healthy adults (ages 36–100+), with each study collecting longitudinal data in a subset of individuals at particular age ranges. The imaging protocols of the HCP-D and HCP-A studies are very similar, differing primarily in the selection of different task-fMRI paradigms. We strove to harmonize the imaging protocol to the greatest extent feasible with the completed HCP-YA (1200+ participants, aged 22–35), but some imaging-related changes were motivated or necessitated by hardware changes, the need to reduce the total amount of scanning per participant, and/or the additional challenges of working with young and elderly populations. Here, we provide an overview of the common HCP-D/A imaging protocol including data and rationales for protocol decisions and changes relative to HCP-YA. The result will be a large, rich, multi-modal, and freely available set of consistently acquired data for use by the scientific community to investigate and define normative developmental and aging related changes in the healthy human brain.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.060

Close

John M. Henderson; Wonil Choi; Steven G. Luke; Joseph Schmidt

Neural correlates of individual differences in fixation duration during natural reading Journal Article

In: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 71, no. 1, pp. 314–323, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Henderson2018,
title = {Neural correlates of individual differences in fixation duration during natural reading},
author = {John M. Henderson and Wonil Choi and Steven G. Luke and Joseph Schmidt},
doi = {10.1080/17470218.2017.1329322},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology},
volume = {71},
number = {1},
pages = {314--323},
abstract = {Reading requires integration of language and cognitive processes with attention and eye movement control. Individuals differ in their reading ability, but little is known about the neurocognitive processes associated with these individual differences. To investigate this issue, we combined eyetracking and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), simultaneously recording eye movements and blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) activity while subjects read text passages. We found that the variability and skew of fixation duration distributions across individuals, as assessed by ex-Gaussian analyses, decreased with increasing neural activity in regions associated with the cortical eye movement control network (left frontal eye fields [FEF], left intraparietal sulcus [IPS] , left inferior frontal gyrus [IFG] and right IFG). The results suggest that individual differences in fixation duration during reading are related to underlying neurocognitive processes associated with the eye movement control system and its relationship to language processing. The results also show that eye movements and fMRI can be combined to investigate the neural correlates of individual differences in natural reading.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Reading requires integration of language and cognitive processes with attention and eye movement control. Individuals differ in their reading ability, but little is known about the neurocognitive processes associated with these individual differences. To investigate this issue, we combined eyetracking and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), simultaneously recording eye movements and blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) activity while subjects read text passages. We found that the variability and skew of fixation duration distributions across individuals, as assessed by ex-Gaussian analyses, decreased with increasing neural activity in regions associated with the cortical eye movement control network (left frontal eye fields [FEF], left intraparietal sulcus [IPS] , left inferior frontal gyrus [IFG] and right IFG). The results suggest that individual differences in fixation duration during reading are related to underlying neurocognitive processes associated with the eye movement control system and its relationship to language processing. The results also show that eye movements and fMRI can be combined to investigate the neural correlates of individual differences in natural reading.

Close

  • doi:10.1080/17470218.2017.1329322

Close

Nora A. Herweg; Tobias Sommer; Nico Bunzeck

Retrieval demands adaptively change striatal old/new signals and boost subsequent long-term memory Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 38, no. 3, pp. 745–754, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Herweg2018,
title = {Retrieval demands adaptively change striatal old/new signals and boost subsequent long-term memory},
author = {Nora A. Herweg and Tobias Sommer and Nico Bunzeck},
doi = {10.1523/jneurosci.1315-17.2017},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {38},
number = {3},
pages = {745--754},
abstract = {The striatum is a central part of the dopaminergic mesolimbic system and contributes both to the encoding and retrieval of long-term memories. In this regard, the co-occurrence of striatal novelty and retrieval success effects in independent studies underlines the structure's double duty and suggests dynamic contextual adaptation. To test this hypothesis and further investigate the underlying mechanisms ofencoding and retrieval dynamics, human subjects viewed pre-familiarized scene images intermixed with new scenes and classified them as indoor versus outdoor (encoding task) or old versus new (retrieval task), while fMRI and eye tracking data were recorded. Subsequently, subjects performed a final recognition task. As hypothesized, striatal activity and pupil size reflected task- conditional salience ofold and new stimuli, but, unexpectedly, this effect was not reflected in the substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area (SN/VTA), medial temporal lobe, or subsequent memory performance. Instead, subsequent memory generally benefitted from retrieval, an effect possibly driven by task difficulty and activity in a network including different parts ofthe striatum and SN/VTA. Our findings extend memory models of encoding and retrieval dynamics by pinpointing a specific contextual factor that differentially modulates the functional properties ofthe mesolimbic system.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The striatum is a central part of the dopaminergic mesolimbic system and contributes both to the encoding and retrieval of long-term memories. In this regard, the co-occurrence of striatal novelty and retrieval success effects in independent studies underlines the structure's double duty and suggests dynamic contextual adaptation. To test this hypothesis and further investigate the underlying mechanisms ofencoding and retrieval dynamics, human subjects viewed pre-familiarized scene images intermixed with new scenes and classified them as indoor versus outdoor (encoding task) or old versus new (retrieval task), while fMRI and eye tracking data were recorded. Subsequently, subjects performed a final recognition task. As hypothesized, striatal activity and pupil size reflected task- conditional salience ofold and new stimuli, but, unexpectedly, this effect was not reflected in the substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area (SN/VTA), medial temporal lobe, or subsequent memory performance. Instead, subsequent memory generally benefitted from retrieval, an effect possibly driven by task difficulty and activity in a network including different parts ofthe striatum and SN/VTA. Our findings extend memory models of encoding and retrieval dynamics by pinpointing a specific contextual factor that differentially modulates the functional properties ofthe mesolimbic system.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1315-17.2017

Close

Michael Jigo; Mengyuan Gong; Taosheng Liu

Neural determinants of task performance during feature-based attention in human cortex Journal Article

In: eNeuro, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 1–15, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Jigo2018,
title = {Neural determinants of task performance during feature-based attention in human cortex},
author = {Michael Jigo and Mengyuan Gong and Taosheng Liu},
doi = {10.1523/eneuro.0375-17.2018},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {eNeuro},
volume = {5},
number = {1},
pages = {1--15},
abstract = {Studies of feature-based attention have associated activity in a dorsal frontoparietal network with putative attentional priority signals. Yet, how this neural activity mediates attentional selection and whether it guides behavior are fundamental questions that require investigation. We reasoned that endogenous fluctuations in the quality of attentional priority should influence task performance. Human subjects detected a speed increment while viewing clockwise (CW) or counterclockwise (CCW) motion (baseline task) or while attending to either direction amid distracters (attention task). In an fMRI experiment, direction-specific neural pattern similarity between the baseline task and the attention task revealed a higher level of similarity for correct than incorrect trials in frontoparietal regions. Using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), we disrupted posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and found a selective deficit in the attention task, but not in the baseline task, demonstrating the necessity of this cortical area during feature-based attention. These results reveal that frontoparietal areas maintain attentional priority that facilitates successful behavioral selection.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Studies of feature-based attention have associated activity in a dorsal frontoparietal network with putative attentional priority signals. Yet, how this neural activity mediates attentional selection and whether it guides behavior are fundamental questions that require investigation. We reasoned that endogenous fluctuations in the quality of attentional priority should influence task performance. Human subjects detected a speed increment while viewing clockwise (CW) or counterclockwise (CCW) motion (baseline task) or while attending to either direction amid distracters (attention task). In an fMRI experiment, direction-specific neural pattern similarity between the baseline task and the attention task revealed a higher level of similarity for correct than incorrect trials in frontoparietal regions. Using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), we disrupted posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and found a selective deficit in the attention task, but not in the baseline task, demonstrating the necessity of this cortical area during feature-based attention. These results reveal that frontoparietal areas maintain attentional priority that facilitates successful behavioral selection.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/eneuro.0375-17.2018

Close

Janne Kauttonen; Yevhen Hlushchuk; Iiro P. Jääskeläinen; Pia Tikka

Brain mechanisms underlying cue-based memorizing during free viewing of movie Memento Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 172, pp. 313–325, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Kauttonen2018,
title = {Brain mechanisms underlying cue-based memorizing during free viewing of movie Memento},
author = {Janne Kauttonen and Yevhen Hlushchuk and Iiro P. Jääskeläinen and Pia Tikka},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.068},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {172},
pages = {313--325},
abstract = {How does the human brain recall and connect relevant memories with unfolding events? To study this, we presented 25 healthy subjects, during functional magnetic resonance imaging, the movie ‘Memento' (director C. Nolan). In this movie, scenes are presented in chronologically reverse order with certain scenes briefly overlapping previously presented scenes. Such overlapping “key-frames” serve as effective memory cues for the viewers, prompting recall of relevant memories of the previously seen scene and connecting them with the concurrent scene. We hypothesized that these repeating key-frames serve as immediate recall cues and would facilitate reconstruction of the story piece-by-piece. The chronological version of Memento, shown in a separate experiment for another group of subjects, served as a control condition. Using multivariate event-related pattern analysis method and representational similarity analysis, focal fingerprint patterns of hemodynamic activity were found to emerge during presentation of key-frame scenes. This effect was present in higher-order cortical network with regions including precuneus, angular gyrus, cingulate gyrus, as well as lateral, superior, and middle frontal gyri within frontal poles. This network was right hemispheric dominant. These distributed patterns of brain activity appear to underlie ability to recall relevant memories and connect them with ongoing events, i.e., “what goes with what” in a complex story. Given the real-life likeness of cinematic experience, these results provide new insight into how the human brain recalls, given proper cues, relevant memories to facilitate understanding and prediction of everyday life events.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

How does the human brain recall and connect relevant memories with unfolding events? To study this, we presented 25 healthy subjects, during functional magnetic resonance imaging, the movie ‘Memento' (director C. Nolan). In this movie, scenes are presented in chronologically reverse order with certain scenes briefly overlapping previously presented scenes. Such overlapping “key-frames” serve as effective memory cues for the viewers, prompting recall of relevant memories of the previously seen scene and connecting them with the concurrent scene. We hypothesized that these repeating key-frames serve as immediate recall cues and would facilitate reconstruction of the story piece-by-piece. The chronological version of Memento, shown in a separate experiment for another group of subjects, served as a control condition. Using multivariate event-related pattern analysis method and representational similarity analysis, focal fingerprint patterns of hemodynamic activity were found to emerge during presentation of key-frame scenes. This effect was present in higher-order cortical network with regions including precuneus, angular gyrus, cingulate gyrus, as well as lateral, superior, and middle frontal gyri within frontal poles. This network was right hemispheric dominant. These distributed patterns of brain activity appear to underlie ability to recall relevant memories and connect them with ongoing events, i.e., “what goes with what” in a complex story. Given the real-life likeness of cinematic experience, these results provide new insight into how the human brain recalls, given proper cues, relevant memories to facilitate understanding and prediction of everyday life events.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.068

Close

Derek Kellar; Sharlene Newman; Franco Pestilli; Hu Cheng; Nicholas L. Port

Comparing fMRI activation during smooth pursuit eye movements among contact sport athletes, non-contact sport athletes, and non-athletes Journal Article

In: NeuroImage: Clinical, vol. 18, pp. 413–424, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Kellar2018,
title = {Comparing fMRI activation during smooth pursuit eye movements among contact sport athletes, non-contact sport athletes, and non-athletes},
author = {Derek Kellar and Sharlene Newman and Franco Pestilli and Hu Cheng and Nicholas L. Port},
doi = {10.1016/j.nicl.2018.01.025},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage: Clinical},
volume = {18},
pages = {413--424},
publisher = {Elsevier},
abstract = {Objectives: Though sub-concussive impacts are common during contact sports, there is little consensus whether repeat blows affect brain function. Using a “lifetime exposure” rather than acute exposure approach, we examined oculomotor performance and brain activation among collegiate football players and two control groups. Our analysis examined whether there are group differences in eye movement behavioral performance and in brain activation during smooth pursuit. Methods: Data from 21 off-season Division I football “starters” were compared with a) 19 collegiate cross-country runners, and b) 11 non-athlete college students who were SES matched to the football player group (total N = 51). Visual smooth pursuit was performed while undergoing fMRI imaging via a 3 Tesla scanner. Smooth pursuit eye movements to three stimulus difficulty levels were measured with regard to RMS error, gain, and lag. Results: No meaningful differences were found for any of the standard analyses used to assess smooth pursuit eye movements. For fMRI, greater activation was seen in the oculomotor region of the cerebellar vermis and areas of the FEF for football players as compared to either control group, who did not differ on any measure. Conclusion: Greater cerebellar activity among football players while performing an oculomotor task could indicate that they are working harder to compensate for some subtle, long-term subconcussive deficits. Alternatively, top athletes in a sport requiring high visual motor skill could have more of their cerebellum and FEF devoted to oculomotor task performance regardless of subconcussive history. Overall, these results provide little firm support for an effect of accumulated subconcussion exposure on brain function.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Objectives: Though sub-concussive impacts are common during contact sports, there is little consensus whether repeat blows affect brain function. Using a “lifetime exposure” rather than acute exposure approach, we examined oculomotor performance and brain activation among collegiate football players and two control groups. Our analysis examined whether there are group differences in eye movement behavioral performance and in brain activation during smooth pursuit. Methods: Data from 21 off-season Division I football “starters” were compared with a) 19 collegiate cross-country runners, and b) 11 non-athlete college students who were SES matched to the football player group (total N = 51). Visual smooth pursuit was performed while undergoing fMRI imaging via a 3 Tesla scanner. Smooth pursuit eye movements to three stimulus difficulty levels were measured with regard to RMS error, gain, and lag. Results: No meaningful differences were found for any of the standard analyses used to assess smooth pursuit eye movements. For fMRI, greater activation was seen in the oculomotor region of the cerebellar vermis and areas of the FEF for football players as compared to either control group, who did not differ on any measure. Conclusion: Greater cerebellar activity among football players while performing an oculomotor task could indicate that they are working harder to compensate for some subtle, long-term subconcussive deficits. Alternatively, top athletes in a sport requiring high visual motor skill could have more of their cerebellum and FEF devoted to oculomotor task performance regardless of subconcussive history. Overall, these results provide little firm support for an effect of accumulated subconcussion exposure on brain function.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2018.01.025

Close

Stephanie J. Larcombe; Christopher Kennard; Holly Bridge

Increase in MST activity correlates with visual motion learning: A functional MRI study of perceptual learning Journal Article

In: Human Brain Mapping, vol. 39, no. 1, pp. 145–156, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Larcombe2018,
title = {Increase in MST activity correlates with visual motion learning: A functional MRI study of perceptual learning},
author = {Stephanie J. Larcombe and Christopher Kennard and Holly Bridge},
doi = {10.1002/hbm.23832},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Human Brain Mapping},
volume = {39},
number = {1},
pages = {145--156},
abstract = {Repeated practice of a specific task can improve visual performance, but the neural mechanisms underlying this improvement in performance are not yet well understood. Here we trained healthy partici- pants on a visual motion task daily for 5 days in one visual hemifield. Before and after training, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure the change in neural activity. We also imaged a control group of participants on two occasions who did not receive any task training. While in the MRI scanner, all participants completed the motion task in the trained and untrained visual hemifields sepa- rately. Following training, participants improved their ability to discriminate motion direction in the trained hemifield and, to a lesser extent, in the untrained hemifield. The amount of task learning correlated positively with the change in activity in the medial superior temporal (MST) area. MST is the anterior por- tion of the human motion complex (hMT1). MST changes were localized to the hemisphere contralateral to the region of the visual field, where perceptual training was delivered. Visual areas V2 and V3a showed an increase in activity between the first and second scan in the training group, but this was not correlated with performance. The contralateral anterior hippocampus and bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and frontal pole showed changes in neural activity that also correlated with the amount of task learning. These findings emphasize the importance of MST in perceptual learning of a visual motion task.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Repeated practice of a specific task can improve visual performance, but the neural mechanisms underlying this improvement in performance are not yet well understood. Here we trained healthy partici- pants on a visual motion task daily for 5 days in one visual hemifield. Before and after training, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure the change in neural activity. We also imaged a control group of participants on two occasions who did not receive any task training. While in the MRI scanner, all participants completed the motion task in the trained and untrained visual hemifields sepa- rately. Following training, participants improved their ability to discriminate motion direction in the trained hemifield and, to a lesser extent, in the untrained hemifield. The amount of task learning correlated positively with the change in activity in the medial superior temporal (MST) area. MST is the anterior por- tion of the human motion complex (hMT1). MST changes were localized to the hemisphere contralateral to the region of the visual field, where perceptual training was delivered. Visual areas V2 and V3a showed an increase in activity between the first and second scan in the training group, but this was not correlated with performance. The contralateral anterior hippocampus and bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and frontal pole showed changes in neural activity that also correlated with the amount of task learning. These findings emphasize the importance of MST in perceptual learning of a visual motion task.

Close

  • doi:10.1002/hbm.23832

Close

Zhong-Xu Liu; Kelly Shen; Rosanna K. Olsen; Jennifer D. Ryan

Age-related changes in the relationship between visual exploration and hippocampal activity Journal Article

In: Neuropsychologia, vol. 119, pp. 81–91, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Liu2018g,
title = {Age-related changes in the relationship between visual exploration and hippocampal activity},
author = {Zhong-Xu Liu and Kelly Shen and Rosanna K. Olsen and Jennifer D. Ryan},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.032},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Neuropsychologia},
volume = {119},
pages = {81--91},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {Deciphering the mechanisms underlying age-related memory declines remains an important goal in cognitive neuroscience. Recently, we observed that visual sampling behavior predicted activity within the hippocampus, a region critical for memory. In younger adults, increases in the number of gaze fixations were associated with increases in hippocampal activity (Liu et al., 2017). This finding suggests a close coupling between the oculomotor and memory system. However, the extent to which this coupling is altered with aging has not been investigated. In this study, we gave older adults the same face processing task used in Liu et al. (2017) and compared their visual exploration behavior and neural activation in the hippocampus and the fusiform face area (FFA) to those of younger adults. Compared to younger adults, older adults showed an increase in visual exploration as indexed by the number of gaze fixations. However, the relationship between visual exploration and neural responses in the hippocampus and FFA was weaker than that of younger adults. Older adults also showed weaker responses to novel faces and a smaller repetition suppression effect in the hippocampus and FFA compared to younger adults. All together, this study provides novel evidence that the capacity to bind visually sampled information, in real-time, into coherent representations along the ventral visual stream and the medial temporal lobe declines with aging.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Deciphering the mechanisms underlying age-related memory declines remains an important goal in cognitive neuroscience. Recently, we observed that visual sampling behavior predicted activity within the hippocampus, a region critical for memory. In younger adults, increases in the number of gaze fixations were associated with increases in hippocampal activity (Liu et al., 2017). This finding suggests a close coupling between the oculomotor and memory system. However, the extent to which this coupling is altered with aging has not been investigated. In this study, we gave older adults the same face processing task used in Liu et al. (2017) and compared their visual exploration behavior and neural activation in the hippocampus and the fusiform face area (FFA) to those of younger adults. Compared to younger adults, older adults showed an increase in visual exploration as indexed by the number of gaze fixations. However, the relationship between visual exploration and neural responses in the hippocampus and FFA was weaker than that of younger adults. Older adults also showed weaker responses to novel faces and a smaller repetition suppression effect in the hippocampus and FFA compared to younger adults. All together, this study provides novel evidence that the capacity to bind visually sampled information, in real-time, into coherent representations along the ventral visual stream and the medial temporal lobe declines with aging.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.032

Close

Kep Kee Loh; Fadila Hadj-Bouziane; Michael Petrides; Emmanuel Procyk; Céline Amiez

Rostro-caudal organization of connectivity between cingulate motor areas and lateral frontal regions Journal Article

In: Frontiers in Neuroscience, vol. 11, pp. 753, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Loh2018,
title = {Rostro-caudal organization of connectivity between cingulate motor areas and lateral frontal regions},
author = {Kep Kee Loh and Fadila Hadj-Bouziane and Michael Petrides and Emmanuel Procyk and Céline Amiez},
doi = {10.3389/fnins.2017.00753},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Frontiers in Neuroscience},
volume = {11},
pages = {753},
abstract = {According to contemporary views, the lateral frontal cortex is organized along a rostro-caudal functional axis with increasingly complex cognitive/behavioral control implemented rostrally, and increasingly detailed motor control implemented caudally. Whether the medial frontal cortex follows the same organization remains to be elucidated. To address this issue, the functional connectivity of the 3 cingulate motor areas (CMAs) in the human brain with the lateral frontal cortex was investigated. First, the CMAs and their representations of hand, tongue, and eye movements were mapped via task-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Second, using resting-state fMRI, their functional connectivity with lateral prefrontal and lateral motor cortical regions of interest (ROIs) were examined. Importantly, the above analyses were conducted at the single-subject level to account for variability in individual cingulate morphology. The results demonstrated a rostro-caudal functional organization of the CMAs in the human brain that parallels that in the lateral frontal cortex: the rostral CMA has stronger functional connectivity with prefrontal regions and weaker connectivity with motor regions; conversely, the more caudal CMAs have weaker prefrontal and stronger motor connectivity. Connectivity patterns of the hand, tongue and eye representations within the CMAs are consistent with that of their parent CMAs. The parallel rostral-to-caudal functional organization observed in the medial and lateral frontal cortex could likely contribute to different hierarchies of cognitive-motor control.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

According to contemporary views, the lateral frontal cortex is organized along a rostro-caudal functional axis with increasingly complex cognitive/behavioral control implemented rostrally, and increasingly detailed motor control implemented caudally. Whether the medial frontal cortex follows the same organization remains to be elucidated. To address this issue, the functional connectivity of the 3 cingulate motor areas (CMAs) in the human brain with the lateral frontal cortex was investigated. First, the CMAs and their representations of hand, tongue, and eye movements were mapped via task-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Second, using resting-state fMRI, their functional connectivity with lateral prefrontal and lateral motor cortical regions of interest (ROIs) were examined. Importantly, the above analyses were conducted at the single-subject level to account for variability in individual cingulate morphology. The results demonstrated a rostro-caudal functional organization of the CMAs in the human brain that parallels that in the lateral frontal cortex: the rostral CMA has stronger functional connectivity with prefrontal regions and weaker connectivity with motor regions; conversely, the more caudal CMAs have weaker prefrontal and stronger motor connectivity. Connectivity patterns of the hand, tongue and eye representations within the CMAs are consistent with that of their parent CMAs. The parallel rostral-to-caudal functional organization observed in the medial and lateral frontal cortex could likely contribute to different hierarchies of cognitive-motor control.

Close

  • doi:10.3389/fnins.2017.00753

Close

Scott Marek; Joshua S. Siegel; Evan M. Gordon; Ryan V. Raut; Caterina Gratton; Dillan J. Newbold; Mario Ortega; Timothy O. Laumann; Babatunde Adeyemo; Derek B. Miller; Annie Zheng; Katherine C. Lopez; Jeffrey J. Berg; Rebecca S. Coalson; Annie L. Nguyen; Donna Dierker; Andrew N. Van; Catherine R. Hoyt; Kathleen B. McDermott; Scott A. Norris; Joshua S. Shimony; Abraham Z. Snyder; Steven M. Nelson; Deanna M. Barch; Bradley L. Schlaggar; Marcus E. Raichle; Steven E. Petersen; Deanna J. Greene; Nico U. F. Dosenbach

Spatial and temporal organization of the individual human cerebellum Journal Article

In: Neuron, vol. 100, no. 4, pp. 977–993.e7, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Marek2018,
title = {Spatial and temporal organization of the individual human cerebellum},
author = {Scott Marek and Joshua S. Siegel and Evan M. Gordon and Ryan V. Raut and Caterina Gratton and Dillan J. Newbold and Mario Ortega and Timothy O. Laumann and Babatunde Adeyemo and Derek B. Miller and Annie Zheng and Katherine C. Lopez and Jeffrey J. Berg and Rebecca S. Coalson and Annie L. Nguyen and Donna Dierker and Andrew N. Van and Catherine R. Hoyt and Kathleen B. McDermott and Scott A. Norris and Joshua S. Shimony and Abraham Z. Snyder and Steven M. Nelson and Deanna M. Barch and Bradley L. Schlaggar and Marcus E. Raichle and Steven E. Petersen and Deanna J. Greene and Nico U. F. Dosenbach},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.010},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Neuron},
volume = {100},
number = {4},
pages = {977--993.e7},
publisher = {Elsevier Inc.},
abstract = {Cerebellar functional networks are topographically individual-specific. Cerebellar intrinsic fMRI signals lag those in cortex by 100–400 ms. The frontoparietal control network is greatly overrepresented (>2-fold), suggesting that the cerebellum is important for the adaptive control of the brain's cognitive processes.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Cerebellar functional networks are topographically individual-specific. Cerebellar intrinsic fMRI signals lag those in cortex by 100–400 ms. The frontoparietal control network is greatly overrepresented (>2-fold), suggesting that the cerebellum is important for the adaptive control of the brain's cognitive processes.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.010

Close

Rachel Millin; Tamar Kolodny; Anastasia V. Flevaris; Alexander M. Kale; Michael Paul Schallmo; Jennifer Gerdts; Raphael A. Bernier; Scott Murray

Reduced auditory cortical adaptation in autism spectrum disorder Journal Article

In: eLife, vol. 7, pp. 1–15, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Millin2018,
title = {Reduced auditory cortical adaptation in autism spectrum disorder},
author = {Rachel Millin and Tamar Kolodny and Anastasia V. Flevaris and Alexander M. Kale and Michael Paul Schallmo and Jennifer Gerdts and Raphael A. Bernier and Scott Murray},
doi = {10.7554/eLife.36493},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {eLife},
volume = {7},
pages = {1--15},
abstract = {

Adaptation is a fundamental property of cortical neurons and has been suggested to be altered in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). We used fMRI to measure adaptation induced by repeated audio-visual stimulation in early sensory cortical areas in individuals with ASD and neurotypical (NT) controls. The initial transient responses were equivalent between groups in both visual and auditory cortices and when stimulation occurred with fixed-interval and randomized-interval timing. However, in auditory but not visual cortex, the post-transient sustained response was greater in individuals with ASD than NT controls in the fixed-interval timing condition, reflecting reduced adaptation. Further, individual differences in the sustained response in auditory cortex correlated with ASD symptom severity. These findings are consistent with hypotheses that ASD is associated with increased neural responsiveness but that responsiveness differences only manifest after repeated stimulation, are specific to the temporal pattern of stimulation, and are confined to specific cortical regions.

},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

<p>Adaptation is a fundamental property of cortical neurons and has been suggested to be altered in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). We used fMRI to measure adaptation induced by repeated audio-visual stimulation in early sensory cortical areas in individuals with ASD and neurotypical (NT) controls. The initial transient responses were equivalent between groups in both visual and auditory cortices and when stimulation occurred with fixed-interval and randomized-interval timing. However, in auditory but not visual cortex, the post-transient sustained response was greater in individuals with ASD than NT controls in the fixed-interval timing condition, reflecting reduced adaptation. Further, individual differences in the sustained response in auditory cortex correlated with ASD symptom severity. These findings are consistent with hypotheses that ASD is associated with increased neural responsiveness but that responsiveness differences only manifest after repeated stimulation, are specific to the temporal pattern of stimulation, and are confined to specific cortical regions.</p>

Close

  • doi:10.7554/eLife.36493

Close

Olga Mishulina; Olga Skripko; Anastasia Korosteleva

Some features of eye movements during reading and retelling the text by people with stuttering Journal Article

In: Procedia Computer Science, vol. 123, pp. 328–333, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Mishulina2018,
title = {Some features of eye movements during reading and retelling the text by people with stuttering},
author = {Olga Mishulina and Olga Skripko and Anastasia Korosteleva},
doi = {10.1016/j.procs.2018.01.051},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Procedia Computer Science},
volume = {123},
pages = {328--333},
publisher = {Elsevier B.V.},
abstract = {The connection between cognitive processes and the movement of the human eye during the reading and retelling the text is investigated. A series of experiments were performed, in which people with normal speech, people with stuttering and in the treatment stage of stuttering took part. The results of the experiment were fixed by the eye tracker and the functional magnetic resonance tomograph. The statistical processing of the tracking data was performed, which discovered stable differences of fixation duration in groups of participants when performing test tasks.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The connection between cognitive processes and the movement of the human eye during the reading and retelling the text is investigated. A series of experiments were performed, in which people with normal speech, people with stuttering and in the treatment stage of stuttering took part. The results of the experiment were fixed by the eye tracker and the functional magnetic resonance tomograph. The statistical processing of the tracking data was performed, which discovered stable differences of fixation duration in groups of participants when performing test tasks.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.procs.2018.01.051

Close

Regine Zopf; Marina Butko; Alexandra Woolgar; Mark A. Williams; Anina N. Rich

Representing the location of manipulable objects in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex: Beyond retinotopic reference frames? Journal Article

In: Cortex, vol. 106, pp. 132–150, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Zopf2018,
title = {Representing the location of manipulable objects in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex: Beyond retinotopic reference frames?},
author = {Regine Zopf and Marina Butko and Alexandra Woolgar and Mark A. Williams and Anina N. Rich},
doi = {10.1016/j.cortex.2018.05.009},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Cortex},
volume = {106},
pages = {132--150},
abstract = {When interacting with objects, we have to represent their location relative to our bodies. To facilitate bodily reactions, location may be encoded in the brain not just with respect to the retina (retinotopic reference frame), but also in relation to the head, trunk or arm (collectively spatiotopic reference frames). While spatiotopic reference frames for location encoding can be found in brain areas for action planning, such as parietal areas, there is debate about the existence of spatiotopic reference frames in higher-level occipitotemporal visual areas. In an extensive multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) fMRI study using faces, headless bodies and scenes stimuli, Golomb and Kanwisher (2012) did not find evidence for spatiotopic reference frames in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex. This finding is important for theories of how stimulus location is encoded in the brain. It is possible, however, that their failure to find spatiotopic reference frames is related to their stimuli: we typically do not manipulate faces, headless bodies or scenes. It is plausible that we only represent body-centred location when viewing objects that are typically manipulated. Here, we tested for object location encoding in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex using manipulable object stimuli (balls and cups) in a MVPA fMRI study. We employed Bayesian analyses to determine sample size and evaluate the sensitivity of our data to test the hypothesis that location can be encoded in a spatiotopic reference frame in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex over the null hypothesis of no spatiotopic location encoding. We found strong evidence for retinotopic location encoding consistent with previous findings that retinotopic reference frames are common neural representations of object location. In contrast, when testing for spatiotopic encoding, we found evidence that object location information for small manipulable objects is not decodable in relation to the body in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex. Post-hoc exploratory analyses suggested that spatiotopic aspects might modulate retinotopic location encoding.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

When interacting with objects, we have to represent their location relative to our bodies. To facilitate bodily reactions, location may be encoded in the brain not just with respect to the retina (retinotopic reference frame), but also in relation to the head, trunk or arm (collectively spatiotopic reference frames). While spatiotopic reference frames for location encoding can be found in brain areas for action planning, such as parietal areas, there is debate about the existence of spatiotopic reference frames in higher-level occipitotemporal visual areas. In an extensive multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) fMRI study using faces, headless bodies and scenes stimuli, Golomb and Kanwisher (2012) did not find evidence for spatiotopic reference frames in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex. This finding is important for theories of how stimulus location is encoded in the brain. It is possible, however, that their failure to find spatiotopic reference frames is related to their stimuli: we typically do not manipulate faces, headless bodies or scenes. It is plausible that we only represent body-centred location when viewing objects that are typically manipulated. Here, we tested for object location encoding in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex using manipulable object stimuli (balls and cups) in a MVPA fMRI study. We employed Bayesian analyses to determine sample size and evaluate the sensitivity of our data to test the hypothesis that location can be encoded in a spatiotopic reference frame in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex over the null hypothesis of no spatiotopic location encoding. We found strong evidence for retinotopic location encoding consistent with previous findings that retinotopic reference frames are common neural representations of object location. In contrast, when testing for spatiotopic encoding, we found evidence that object location information for small manipulable objects is not decodable in relation to the body in shape-selective occipitotemporal cortex. Post-hoc exploratory analyses suggested that spatiotopic aspects might modulate retinotopic location encoding.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2018.05.009

Close

Iske Bakker-Marshall; Atsuko Takashima; Jan-Mathijs Schoffelen; Janet G. Hell; Gabriele Janzen; James M. McQueen

Theta-band oscillations in the middle temporal gyrus reflect novel word consolidation Journal Article

In: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 5, pp. 621–633, 2018.

Abstract | BibTeX

@article{BakkerMarshall2018,
title = {Theta-band oscillations in the middle temporal gyrus reflect novel word consolidation},
author = {Iske Bakker-Marshall and Atsuko Takashima and Jan-Mathijs Schoffelen and Janet G. Hell and Gabriele Janzen and James M. McQueen},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
volume = {30},
number = {5},
pages = {621--633},
abstract = {Like many other types of memory formation, novel word learning benefits from an offline consolidation period after the initial encoding phase. A previous EEG study has shown that retrieval of novel words elicited more word-like-induced electrophysiological brain activity in the theta band after consolidation [Bakker, I., Takashima, A., van Hell, J. G., Janzen, G., & McQueen, J. M. Changes in theta and beta oscillations as signatures of novel word consolidation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1286–1297, 2015]. This suggests that theta-band oscillations play a role in lexicalization, but it has not been demonstrated that this effect is directly caused by the formation of lexical representations. This study used magnetoencephalography to localize the theta consolidation effect to the left posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG), a region known to be involved in lexical storage. Both untrained novel words and words learned immediately before test elicited lower theta power during retrieval than existing words in this region. After a 24-hr consolidation period, the difference between novel and existing words decreased significantly, most strongly in the left pMTG. The magnitude of the decrease after consolidation correlated with an increase in behavioral competition effects between novel words and existing words with similar spelling, reflecting functional integration into the mental lexicon. These results thus provide new evidence that consolidation aids the development of lexical representations mediated by the left pMTG. Theta synchronizationmay enable lexical access by facilitating the simultaneous activation of distributed semantic, phonological, and orthographic representations that are bound together in the pMTG.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Like many other types of memory formation, novel word learning benefits from an offline consolidation period after the initial encoding phase. A previous EEG study has shown that retrieval of novel words elicited more word-like-induced electrophysiological brain activity in the theta band after consolidation [Bakker, I., Takashima, A., van Hell, J. G., Janzen, G., & McQueen, J. M. Changes in theta and beta oscillations as signatures of novel word consolidation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 1286–1297, 2015]. This suggests that theta-band oscillations play a role in lexicalization, but it has not been demonstrated that this effect is directly caused by the formation of lexical representations. This study used magnetoencephalography to localize the theta consolidation effect to the left posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG), a region known to be involved in lexical storage. Both untrained novel words and words learned immediately before test elicited lower theta power during retrieval than existing words in this region. After a 24-hr consolidation period, the difference between novel and existing words decreased significantly, most strongly in the left pMTG. The magnitude of the decrease after consolidation correlated with an increase in behavioral competition effects between novel words and existing words with similar spelling, reflecting functional integration into the mental lexicon. These results thus provide new evidence that consolidation aids the development of lexical representations mediated by the left pMTG. Theta synchronizationmay enable lexical access by facilitating the simultaneous activation of distributed semantic, phonological, and orthographic representations that are bound together in the pMTG.

Close

Eran Eldar; Gyung Jin Bae; Zeb Kurth-Nelson; Peter Dayan; Raymond J. Dolan

Magnetoencephalography decoding reveals structural differences within integrative decision processes Journal Article

In: Nature Human Behaviour, vol. 2, no. 9, pp. 670–681, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Eldar2018,
title = {Magnetoencephalography decoding reveals structural differences within integrative decision processes},
author = {Eran Eldar and Gyung Jin Bae and Zeb Kurth-Nelson and Peter Dayan and Raymond J. Dolan},
doi = {10.1038/s41562-018-0423-3},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Nature Human Behaviour},
volume = {2},
number = {9},
pages = {670--681},
publisher = {Springer US},
abstract = {When confronted with complex inputs consisting of multiple elements, humans use various strategies to integrate the elements quickly and accurately. For instance, accuracy may or over be improved by processing elements one at a time1–4 extended periods5–8 ; speed can increase if the internal rep- resentation of elements is accelerated9,10 . However, little is known about how humans actually approach these challenges because behavioural findings can be accounted for by mul- tiple alternative process models11 and neuroimaging investi-gations typically rely on haemodynamic signals that change too slowly. Consequently, to uncover the fast neural dynamics that support information integration, we decoded magnetoencephalographic signals that were recorded as human subjects performed a complex decision task. Our findings reveal three sources of individual differences in the temporal structure of the integration process—sequential representation, partial reinstatement and early computation—each having a dissociable effect on how subjects handled problem complexity and temporal constraints. Our findings shed new light on the structure and influence of self-determined neural integration processes.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

When confronted with complex inputs consisting of multiple elements, humans use various strategies to integrate the elements quickly and accurately. For instance, accuracy may or over be improved by processing elements one at a time1–4 extended periods5–8 ; speed can increase if the internal rep- resentation of elements is accelerated9,10 . However, little is known about how humans actually approach these challenges because behavioural findings can be accounted for by mul- tiple alternative process models11 and neuroimaging investi-gations typically rely on haemodynamic signals that change too slowly. Consequently, to uncover the fast neural dynamics that support information integration, we decoded magnetoencephalographic signals that were recorded as human subjects performed a complex decision task. Our findings reveal three sources of individual differences in the temporal structure of the integration process—sequential representation, partial reinstatement and early computation—each having a dissociable effect on how subjects handled problem complexity and temporal constraints. Our findings shed new light on the structure and influence of self-determined neural integration processes.

Close

  • doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0423-3

Close

Wei He; Blake W. Johnson

Development of face recognition: Dynamic causal modelling of MEG data Journal Article

In: Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 30, pp. 13–22, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{He2018,
title = {Development of face recognition: Dynamic causal modelling of MEG data},
author = {Wei He and Blake W. Johnson},
doi = {10.1016/j.dcn.2017.11.010},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience},
volume = {30},
pages = {13--22},
publisher = {Elsevier},
abstract = {Electrophysiological studies of adults indicate that brain activity is enhanced during viewing of repeated faces, at a latency of about 250 ms after the onset of the face (M250/N250). The present study aimed to determine if this effect was also present in preschool-aged children, whose brain activity was measured in a custom-sized pediatric MEG system. The results showed that, unlike adults, face repetition did not show any significant modulation of M250 amplitude in children; however children's M250 latencies were significantly faster for repeated than non-repeated faces. Dynamic causal modelling (DCM) of the M250 in both age groups tested the effects of face repetition within the core face network including the occipital face area (OFA), the fusiform face area (FFA), and the superior temporal sulcus (STS). DCM revealed that repetition of identical faces altered both forward and backward connections in children and adults; however the modulations involved inputs to both FFA and OFA in adults but only to OFA in children. These findings suggest that the amplitude-insensitivity of the immature M250 may be due to a weaker connection between the FFA and lower visual areas.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Electrophysiological studies of adults indicate that brain activity is enhanced during viewing of repeated faces, at a latency of about 250 ms after the onset of the face (M250/N250). The present study aimed to determine if this effect was also present in preschool-aged children, whose brain activity was measured in a custom-sized pediatric MEG system. The results showed that, unlike adults, face repetition did not show any significant modulation of M250 amplitude in children; however children's M250 latencies were significantly faster for repeated than non-repeated faces. Dynamic causal modelling (DCM) of the M250 in both age groups tested the effects of face repetition within the core face network including the occipital face area (OFA), the fusiform face area (FFA), and the superior temporal sulcus (STS). DCM revealed that repetition of identical faces altered both forward and backward connections in children and adults; however the modulations involved inputs to both FFA and OFA in adults but only to OFA in children. These findings suggest that the amplitude-insensitivity of the immature M250 may be due to a weaker connection between the FFA and lower visual areas.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2017.11.010

Close

Simone G. Heideman; Gustavo Rohenkohl; Joshua J. Chauvin; Clare E. Palmer; Freek Ede; Anna C. Nobre

Anticipatory neural dynamics of spatial-temporal orienting of attention in younger and older adults Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 178, pp. 46–56, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Heideman2018a,
title = {Anticipatory neural dynamics of spatial-temporal orienting of attention in younger and older adults},
author = {Simone G. Heideman and Gustavo Rohenkohl and Joshua J. Chauvin and Clare E. Palmer and Freek Ede and Anna C. Nobre},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.002},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {178},
pages = {46--56},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd},
abstract = {Spatial and temporal expectations act synergistically to facilitate visual perception. In the current study, we sought to investigate the anticipatory oscillatory markers of combined spatial-temporal orienting and to test whether these decline with ageing. We examined anticipatory neural dynamics associated with joint spatial-temporal orienting of attention using magnetoencephalography (MEG) in both younger and older adults. Participants performed a cued covert spatial-temporal orienting task requiring the discrimination of a visual target. Cues indicated both where and when targets would appear. In both age groups, valid spatial-temporal cues significantly enhanced perceptual sensitivity and reduced reaction times. In the MEG data, the main effect of spatial orienting was the lateralised anticipatory modulation of posterior alpha and beta oscillations. In contrast to previous reports, this modulation was not attenuated in older adults; instead it was even more pronounced. The main effect of temporal orienting was a bilateral suppression of posterior alpha and beta oscillations. This effect was restricted to younger adults. Our results also revealed a striking interaction between anticipatory spatial and temporal orienting in the gamma-band (60–75 Hz). When considering both age groups separately, this effect was only clearly evident and only survived statistical evaluation in the older adults. Together, these observations provide several new insights into the neural dynamics supporting separate as well as combined effects of spatial and temporal orienting of attention, and suggest that different neural dynamics associated with attentional orienting appear differentially sensitive to ageing.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Spatial and temporal expectations act synergistically to facilitate visual perception. In the current study, we sought to investigate the anticipatory oscillatory markers of combined spatial-temporal orienting and to test whether these decline with ageing. We examined anticipatory neural dynamics associated with joint spatial-temporal orienting of attention using magnetoencephalography (MEG) in both younger and older adults. Participants performed a cued covert spatial-temporal orienting task requiring the discrimination of a visual target. Cues indicated both where and when targets would appear. In both age groups, valid spatial-temporal cues significantly enhanced perceptual sensitivity and reduced reaction times. In the MEG data, the main effect of spatial orienting was the lateralised anticipatory modulation of posterior alpha and beta oscillations. In contrast to previous reports, this modulation was not attenuated in older adults; instead it was even more pronounced. The main effect of temporal orienting was a bilateral suppression of posterior alpha and beta oscillations. This effect was restricted to younger adults. Our results also revealed a striking interaction between anticipatory spatial and temporal orienting in the gamma-band (60–75 Hz). When considering both age groups separately, this effect was only clearly evident and only survived statistical evaluation in the older adults. Together, these observations provide several new insights into the neural dynamics supporting separate as well as combined effects of spatial and temporal orienting of attention, and suggest that different neural dynamics associated with attentional orienting appear differentially sensitive to ageing.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.002

Close

Simone G. Heideman; Freek Ede; Anna C. Nobre

Temporal alignment of anticipatory motor cortical beta lateralisation in hidden visual-motor sequences Journal Article

In: European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 48, no. 8, pp. 2684–2695, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Heideman2018,
title = {Temporal alignment of anticipatory motor cortical beta lateralisation in hidden visual-motor sequences},
author = {Simone G. Heideman and Freek Ede and Anna C. Nobre},
doi = {10.1111/ejn.13700},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {European Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {48},
number = {8},
pages = {2684--2695},
abstract = {Performance improves when participants respond to events that are structured in repeating sequences, suggesting that learning can lead to proactive anticipatory preparation. Whereas most sequence-learning studies have emphasised spatial structure, most sequences also contain a prominent temporal structure. We used MEG to investigate spatial and temporal anticipatory neural dynamics in a modified serial reaction time (SRT) task. Performance and brain activity were compared between blocks with learned spatial-temporal sequences and blocks with new sequences. After confirming a strong behavioural benefit of spatial-temporal predictability, we show lateralisation of beta oscillations in anticipation of the response associated with the upcoming target location and show that this also aligns to the expected timing of these forthcoming events. This effect was found both when comparing between repeated (learned) and new (unlearned) sequences, as well as when comparing targets that were expected after short vs. long intervals within the repeated (learned) sequence. Our findings suggest that learning of spatial-temporal structure leads to proactive and dynamic modulation of motor cortical excitability in anticipation of both the location and timing of events that are relevant to guide action.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Performance improves when participants respond to events that are structured in repeating sequences, suggesting that learning can lead to proactive anticipatory preparation. Whereas most sequence-learning studies have emphasised spatial structure, most sequences also contain a prominent temporal structure. We used MEG to investigate spatial and temporal anticipatory neural dynamics in a modified serial reaction time (SRT) task. Performance and brain activity were compared between blocks with learned spatial-temporal sequences and blocks with new sequences. After confirming a strong behavioural benefit of spatial-temporal predictability, we show lateralisation of beta oscillations in anticipation of the response associated with the upcoming target location and show that this also aligns to the expected timing of these forthcoming events. This effect was found both when comparing between repeated (learned) and new (unlearned) sequences, as well as when comparing targets that were expected after short vs. long intervals within the repeated (learned) sequence. Our findings suggest that learning of spatial-temporal structure leads to proactive and dynamic modulation of motor cortical excitability in anticipation of both the location and timing of events that are relevant to guide action.

Close

  • doi:10.1111/ejn.13700

Close

Simone G. Heideman; Freek Ede; Anna C. Nobre

Early behavioural facilitation by temporal expectations in complex visual-motor sequences Journal Article

In: Neuroscience, vol. 389, pp. 74–84, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Heideman2018b,
title = {Early behavioural facilitation by temporal expectations in complex visual-motor sequences},
author = {Simone G. Heideman and Freek Ede and Anna C. Nobre},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.05.014},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Neuroscience},
volume = {389},
pages = {74--84},
abstract = {In daily life, temporal expectations may derive from incidental learning of recurring patterns of intervals. We investigated the incidental acquisition and utilisation of combined temporal-ordinal (spatial/effector) structure in complex visual-motor sequences using a modified version of a serial reaction time (SRT) task. In this task, not only the series of targets/responses, but also the series of intervals between subsequent targets was repeated across multiple presentations of the same sequence. Each participant completed three sessions. In the first session, only the repeating sequence was presented. During the second and third session, occasional probe blocks were presented, where a new (unlearned) spatial-temporal sequence was introduced. We first confirm that participants not only got faster over time, but that they were slower and less accurate during probe blocks, indicating that they incidentally learned the sequence structure. Having established a robust behavioural benefit induced by the repeating spatial-temporal sequence, we next addressed our central hypothesis that implicit temporal orienting (evoked by the learned temporal structure) would have the largest influence on performance for targets following short (as opposed to longer) intervals between temporally structured sequence elements, paralleling classical observations in tasks using explicit temporal cues. We found that indeed, reaction time differences between new and repeated sequences were largest for the short interval, compared to the medium and long intervals, and that this was the case, even when comparing late blocks (where the repeated sequence had been incidentally learned), to early blocks (where this sequence was still unfamiliar). We conclude that incidentally acquired temporal expectations that follow a sequential structure can have a robust facilitatory influence on visually-guided behavioural responses and that, like more explicit forms of temporal orienting, this effect is most pronounced for sequence elements that are expected at short inter-element intervals.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

In daily life, temporal expectations may derive from incidental learning of recurring patterns of intervals. We investigated the incidental acquisition and utilisation of combined temporal-ordinal (spatial/effector) structure in complex visual-motor sequences using a modified version of a serial reaction time (SRT) task. In this task, not only the series of targets/responses, but also the series of intervals between subsequent targets was repeated across multiple presentations of the same sequence. Each participant completed three sessions. In the first session, only the repeating sequence was presented. During the second and third session, occasional probe blocks were presented, where a new (unlearned) spatial-temporal sequence was introduced. We first confirm that participants not only got faster over time, but that they were slower and less accurate during probe blocks, indicating that they incidentally learned the sequence structure. Having established a robust behavioural benefit induced by the repeating spatial-temporal sequence, we next addressed our central hypothesis that implicit temporal orienting (evoked by the learned temporal structure) would have the largest influence on performance for targets following short (as opposed to longer) intervals between temporally structured sequence elements, paralleling classical observations in tasks using explicit temporal cues. We found that indeed, reaction time differences between new and repeated sequences were largest for the short interval, compared to the medium and long intervals, and that this was the case, even when comparing late blocks (where the repeated sequence had been incidentally learned), to early blocks (where this sequence was still unfamiliar). We conclude that incidentally acquired temporal expectations that follow a sequential structure can have a robust facilitatory influence on visually-guided behavioural responses and that, like more explicit forms of temporal orienting, this effect is most pronounced for sequence elements that are expected at short inter-element intervals.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.05.014

Close

Carina Kelbsch; Archana Jalligampala; Torsten Strasser; Paul Richter; Katarina Stingl; Christoph Braun; Daniel L. Rathbun; Eberhart Zrenner; Helmut Wilhelm; Barbara Wilhelm; Tobias Peters; Krunoslav Stingl

Phosphene perception and pupillary responses to sinusoidal electrostimulation - For an objective measurement of retinal function Journal Article

In: Experimental Eye Research, vol. 176, pp. 210–218, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Kelbsch2018,
title = {Phosphene perception and pupillary responses to sinusoidal electrostimulation - For an objective measurement of retinal function},
author = {Carina Kelbsch and Archana Jalligampala and Torsten Strasser and Paul Richter and Katarina Stingl and Christoph Braun and Daniel L. Rathbun and Eberhart Zrenner and Helmut Wilhelm and Barbara Wilhelm and Tobias Peters and Krunoslav Stingl},
doi = {10.1016/j.exer.2018.07.010},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Experimental Eye Research},
volume = {176},
pages = {210--218},
publisher = {Elsevier},
abstract = {The purpose was to evaluate retinal function by measuring pupillary responses to sinusoidal transcorneal electrostimulation in healthy young human subjects. This work also translates data from analogous in vitro experiments and connects it to the pupillary responses obtained in human experiments. 14 healthy human subjects participated (4 males, 10 females); for the in vitro experiments, two male healthy mouse retinas (adult wild-type C57B/6J) were used. Pupillary responses to sinusoidal transcorneal electrostimulation of varying stimulus carrier frequencies (10, 20 Hz; envelope frequency constantly kept at 1.2 Hz) and intensities (10, 20, 50 $mu$A) were recorded and compared with those obtained with light stimulation (1.2 Hz sinusoidal blue, red light). A strong correlation between the sinusoidal stimulation (electrical as well as light) and the pupillary sinusoidal response was found. The difference between the lag of electrical and light stimulation allowed the estimation of an intensity threshold for pupillary responses to transcorneal electrostimulation (mean ± SD: 30 ± 10 $mu$A (10 Hz); 38 ± 10 $mu$A (20 Hz)). A comparison between the results of the two stimulation frequencies showed a not statistically significant smaller lag for 10 Hz (10 Hz: 633 ± 90 ms; 20 Hz: 725 ± 178 ms; 50 $mu$A intensity). Analogous in vitro experiments on murine retinas indicated a selective stimulation of photoreceptors and bipolar cells (lower frequencies) and retinal ganglion cells (higher frequencies) and lower stimulation thresholds for the retinal network with sinusoidal compared to pulsatile stimulation – emphasizing that sinu- soidal waveforms are well-suited to our purposes. We demonstrate that pupillary responses to sinusoidal transcorneal electrostimulation are measurable as an objective marker in healthy young subjects, even at very low stimulus intensities. By using this unique approach, we unveil the potential for an estimation of the in- dividual intensity threshold and a selective activation of different retinal cell types in humans by varying the stimulation frequency. This technique may have broad clinical utility as well as specific relevance in the monitoring of patients with hereditary retinal disorders, especially as implemented in study protocols for novel therapies, e.g. retinal prostheses or gene therapies.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The purpose was to evaluate retinal function by measuring pupillary responses to sinusoidal transcorneal electrostimulation in healthy young human subjects. This work also translates data from analogous in vitro experiments and connects it to the pupillary responses obtained in human experiments. 14 healthy human subjects participated (4 males, 10 females); for the in vitro experiments, two male healthy mouse retinas (adult wild-type C57B/6J) were used. Pupillary responses to sinusoidal transcorneal electrostimulation of varying stimulus carrier frequencies (10, 20 Hz; envelope frequency constantly kept at 1.2 Hz) and intensities (10, 20, 50 $mu$A) were recorded and compared with those obtained with light stimulation (1.2 Hz sinusoidal blue, red light). A strong correlation between the sinusoidal stimulation (electrical as well as light) and the pupillary sinusoidal response was found. The difference between the lag of electrical and light stimulation allowed the estimation of an intensity threshold for pupillary responses to transcorneal electrostimulation (mean ± SD: 30 ± 10 $mu$A (10 Hz); 38 ± 10 $mu$A (20 Hz)). A comparison between the results of the two stimulation frequencies showed a not statistically significant smaller lag for 10 Hz (10 Hz: 633 ± 90 ms; 20 Hz: 725 ± 178 ms; 50 $mu$A intensity). Analogous in vitro experiments on murine retinas indicated a selective stimulation of photoreceptors and bipolar cells (lower frequencies) and retinal ganglion cells (higher frequencies) and lower stimulation thresholds for the retinal network with sinusoidal compared to pulsatile stimulation – emphasizing that sinu- soidal waveforms are well-suited to our purposes. We demonstrate that pupillary responses to sinusoidal transcorneal electrostimulation are measurable as an objective marker in healthy young subjects, even at very low stimulus intensities. By using this unique approach, we unveil the potential for an estimation of the in- dividual intensity threshold and a selective activation of different retinal cell types in humans by varying the stimulation frequency. This technique may have broad clinical utility as well as specific relevance in the monitoring of patients with hereditary retinal disorders, especially as implemented in study protocols for novel therapies, e.g. retinal prostheses or gene therapies.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.exer.2018.07.010

Close

Eline R. Kupers; Helena X. Wang; Kaoru Amano; Kendrick N. Kay; David J. Heeger; Jonathan Winawer

A non-invasive, quantitative study of broadband spectral responses in human visual cortex Journal Article

In: PLoS ONE, vol. 13, no. 3, pp. e0193107, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Kupers2018,
title = {A non-invasive, quantitative study of broadband spectral responses in human visual cortex},
author = {Eline R. Kupers and Helena X. Wang and Kaoru Amano and Kendrick N. Kay and David J. Heeger and Jonathan Winawer},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0193107},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {PLoS ONE},
volume = {13},
number = {3},
pages = {e0193107},
abstract = {Currently, non-invasive methods for studying the human brain do not routinely and reliably measure spike-rate-dependent signals, independent of responses such as hemodynamic coupling (fMRI) and subthreshold neuronal synchrony (oscillations and event-related potentials). In contrast, invasive methods-microelectrode recordings and electrocorticography (ECoG)-have recently measured broadband power elevation in field potentials ($sim$50-200 Hz) as a proxy for locally averaged spike rates. Here, we sought to detect and quantify stimulus-related broadband responses using magnetoencephalography (MEG). Extracranial measurements like MEG and EEG have multiple global noise sources and relatively low signal-to-noise ratios; moreover high frequency artifacts from eye movements can be confounded with stimulus design and mistaken for signals originating from brain activity. For these reasons, we developed an automated denoising technique that helps reveal the broadband signal of interest. Subjects viewed 12-Hz contrast-reversing patterns in the left, right, or bilateral visual field. Sensor time series were separated into evoked (12-Hz amplitude) and broadband components (60-150 Hz). In all subjects, denoised broadband responses were reliably measured in sensors over occipital cortex, even in trials without microsaccades. The broadband pattern was stimulus-dependent, with greater power contralateral to the stimulus. Because we obtain reliable broadband estimates with short experiments ($sim$20 minutes), and with sufficient signal-to-noise to distinguish responses to different stimuli, we conclude that MEG broadband signals, denoised with our method, offer a practical, non-invasive means for characterizing spike-rate-dependent neural activity for addressing scientific questions about human brain function.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Currently, non-invasive methods for studying the human brain do not routinely and reliably measure spike-rate-dependent signals, independent of responses such as hemodynamic coupling (fMRI) and subthreshold neuronal synchrony (oscillations and event-related potentials). In contrast, invasive methods-microelectrode recordings and electrocorticography (ECoG)-have recently measured broadband power elevation in field potentials ($sim$50-200 Hz) as a proxy for locally averaged spike rates. Here, we sought to detect and quantify stimulus-related broadband responses using magnetoencephalography (MEG). Extracranial measurements like MEG and EEG have multiple global noise sources and relatively low signal-to-noise ratios; moreover high frequency artifacts from eye movements can be confounded with stimulus design and mistaken for signals originating from brain activity. For these reasons, we developed an automated denoising technique that helps reveal the broadband signal of interest. Subjects viewed 12-Hz contrast-reversing patterns in the left, right, or bilateral visual field. Sensor time series were separated into evoked (12-Hz amplitude) and broadband components (60-150 Hz). In all subjects, denoised broadband responses were reliably measured in sensors over occipital cortex, even in trials without microsaccades. The broadband pattern was stimulus-dependent, with greater power contralateral to the stimulus. Because we obtain reliable broadband estimates with short experiments ($sim$20 minutes), and with sufficient signal-to-noise to distinguish responses to different stimuli, we conclude that MEG broadband signals, denoised with our method, offer a practical, non-invasive means for characterizing spike-rate-dependent neural activity for addressing scientific questions about human brain function.

Close

  • doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0193107

Close

Mariya E. Manahova; Pim Mostert; Peter Kok; Jan-Mathijs Schoffelen; Floris P. Lange

Stimulus familiarity and expectation jointly modulate neural activity in the visual ventral stream Journal Article

In: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 9, pp. 1366–1377, 2018.

Abstract | BibTeX

@article{Manahova2018,
title = {Stimulus familiarity and expectation jointly modulate neural activity in the visual ventral stream},
author = {Mariya E. Manahova and Pim Mostert and Peter Kok and Jan-Mathijs Schoffelen and Floris P. Lange},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
volume = {30},
number = {9},
pages = {1366--1377},
abstract = {Prior knowledge about the visual world can change how a visual stimulus is processed. Two forms of prior knowledge are often distinguished: stimulus familiarity (i.e., whether a stimulus has been seen before) and stimulus expectation (i.e., whether a stimulus is expected to occur, based on the context). Neurophysiological studies in monkeys have shown suppression of spiking activity both for expected and for familiar items in object-selective inferotemporal cortex. It is an open question, however, if and how these types of knowledge interact in their modulatory effects on the sensory response. To address this issue and to examine whether previous findings generalize to noninvasively measured neural activity in humans, we separately manipulated stimulus familiarity and expectation while noninvasively recording human brain activity using magnetoencephalography. We observed independent suppression of neural activity by familiarity and expectation, specifically in the lateral occipital complex, the putative human homologue of monkey inferotemporal cortex. Familiarity also led to sharpened response dynamics, which was predominantly observed in early visual cortex. Together, these results show that distinct types of sensory knowledge jointly determine the amount of neural resources dedicated to object processing in the visual ventral stream.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Prior knowledge about the visual world can change how a visual stimulus is processed. Two forms of prior knowledge are often distinguished: stimulus familiarity (i.e., whether a stimulus has been seen before) and stimulus expectation (i.e., whether a stimulus is expected to occur, based on the context). Neurophysiological studies in monkeys have shown suppression of spiking activity both for expected and for familiar items in object-selective inferotemporal cortex. It is an open question, however, if and how these types of knowledge interact in their modulatory effects on the sensory response. To address this issue and to examine whether previous findings generalize to noninvasively measured neural activity in humans, we separately manipulated stimulus familiarity and expectation while noninvasively recording human brain activity using magnetoencephalography. We observed independent suppression of neural activity by familiarity and expectation, specifically in the lateral occipital complex, the putative human homologue of monkey inferotemporal cortex. Familiarity also led to sharpened response dynamics, which was predominantly observed in early visual cortex. Together, these results show that distinct types of sensory knowledge jointly determine the amount of neural resources dedicated to object processing in the visual ventral stream.

Close

Pim Mostert; Anke Marit Albers; Loek Brinkman; Larisa Todorova; Peter Kok; Floris P. Lange

Eye movement-related confounds in neural decoding of visual working memory representations Journal Article

In: eNeuro, vol. 5, no. 4, pp. 1–14, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Mostert2018a,
title = {Eye movement-related confounds in neural decoding of visual working memory representations},
author = {Pim Mostert and Anke Marit Albers and Loek Brinkman and Larisa Todorova and Peter Kok and Floris P. Lange},
doi = {10.1523/eneuro.0401-17.2018},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {eNeuro},
volume = {5},
number = {4},
pages = {1--14},
abstract = {A relatively new analysis technique, known as neural decoding or multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA), has become increasingly popular for cognitive neuroimaging studies over recent years. These techniques promise to uncover the representational contents of neural signals, as well as the underlying code and the dynamic profile thereof. A field in which these techniques have led to novel insights in particular is that of visual working memory (VWM). In the present study, we subjected human volunteers to a combined VWM/imagery task while recording their neural signals using magnetoencephalography (MEG). We applied multivariate decoding analyses to uncover the temporal profile underlying the neural representations of the memorized item. Analysis of gaze position however revealed that our results were contaminated by systematic eye movements, suggesting that the MEG decoding results from our originally planned analyses were confounded. In addition to the eye movement analyses, we also present the original analyses to highlight how these might have readily led to invalid conclusions. Finally, we demonstrate a potential remedy, whereby we train the decoders on a functional localizer that was specifically designed to target bottom-up sensory signals and as such avoids eye movements. We conclude by arguing for more awareness of the potentially pervasive and ubiquitous effects of eye movement-related confounds.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

A relatively new analysis technique, known as neural decoding or multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA), has become increasingly popular for cognitive neuroimaging studies over recent years. These techniques promise to uncover the representational contents of neural signals, as well as the underlying code and the dynamic profile thereof. A field in which these techniques have led to novel insights in particular is that of visual working memory (VWM). In the present study, we subjected human volunteers to a combined VWM/imagery task while recording their neural signals using magnetoencephalography (MEG). We applied multivariate decoding analyses to uncover the temporal profile underlying the neural representations of the memorized item. Analysis of gaze position however revealed that our results were contaminated by systematic eye movements, suggesting that the MEG decoding results from our originally planned analyses were confounded. In addition to the eye movement analyses, we also present the original analyses to highlight how these might have readily led to invalid conclusions. Finally, we demonstrate a potential remedy, whereby we train the decoders on a functional localizer that was specifically designed to target bottom-up sensory signals and as such avoids eye movements. We conclude by arguing for more awareness of the potentially pervasive and ubiquitous effects of eye movement-related confounds.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/eneuro.0401-17.2018

Close

Pim Mostert; Sander Bosch; Nadine Dijkstra; Marcel A. J. Gerven; Floris P. Lange

Differential temporal dynamics during visual imagery and perception Journal Article

In: eLife, vol. 7, pp. 1–16, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Mostert2018,
title = {Differential temporal dynamics during visual imagery and perception},
author = {Pim Mostert and Sander Bosch and Nadine Dijkstra and Marcel A. J. Gerven and Floris P. Lange},
doi = {10.7554/elife.33904},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {eLife},
volume = {7},
pages = {1--16},
abstract = {Visual perception and imagery rely on similar representations in the visual cortex. During perception, visual activity is characterized by distinct processing stages, but the temporal dynamics underlying imagery remain unclear. Here, we investigated the dynamics of visual imagery in human participants using magnetoencephalography. Firstly, we show that, compared to perception, imagery decoding becomes significant later and representations at the start of imagery already overlap with later time points. This suggests that during imagery, the entire visual representation is activated at once or that there are large differences in the timing of imagery between trials. Secondly, we found consistent overlap between imagery and perceptual processing around 160 ms and from 300 ms after stimulus onset. This indicates that the N170 gets reactivated during imagery and that imagery does not rely on early perceptual representations. Together, these results provide important insights for our understanding of the neural mechanisms of visual imagery.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Visual perception and imagery rely on similar representations in the visual cortex. During perception, visual activity is characterized by distinct processing stages, but the temporal dynamics underlying imagery remain unclear. Here, we investigated the dynamics of visual imagery in human participants using magnetoencephalography. Firstly, we show that, compared to perception, imagery decoding becomes significant later and representations at the start of imagery already overlap with later time points. This suggests that during imagery, the entire visual representation is activated at once or that there are large differences in the timing of imagery between trials. Secondly, we found consistent overlap between imagery and perceptual processing around 160 ms and from 300 ms after stimulus onset. This indicates that the N170 gets reactivated during imagery and that imagery does not rely on early perceptual representations. Together, these results provide important insights for our understanding of the neural mechanisms of visual imagery.

Close

  • doi:10.7554/elife.33904

Close

Elena V. Orekhova; Olga V. Sysoeva; Justin F. Schneiderman; Sebastian Lundström; Ilia A. Galuta; Dzerasa E. Goiaeva; Andrey O. Prokofyev; Bushra Riaz; Courtney Keeler; Nouchine Hadjikhani; Christopher Gillberg; Tatiana A. Stroganova

Input-dependent modulation of MEG gamma oscillations reflects gain control in the visual cortex Journal Article

In: Scientific Reports, vol. 8, pp. 8451, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Orekhova2018,
title = {Input-dependent modulation of MEG gamma oscillations reflects gain control in the visual cortex},
author = {Elena V. Orekhova and Olga V. Sysoeva and Justin F. Schneiderman and Sebastian Lundström and Ilia A. Galuta and Dzerasa E. Goiaeva and Andrey O. Prokofyev and Bushra Riaz and Courtney Keeler and Nouchine Hadjikhani and Christopher Gillberg and Tatiana A. Stroganova},
doi = {10.1038/s41598-018-26779-6},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {8},
pages = {8451},
abstract = {Gamma-band oscillations arise from the interplay between neural excitation (E) and inhibition (I) and may provide a non-invasive window into the state of cortical circuitry. A bell-shaped modulation of gamma response power by increasing the intensity of sensory input was observed in animals and is thought to reflect neural gain control. Here we sought to find a similar input-output relationship in humans with MEG via modulating the intensity of a visual stimulation by changing the velocity/ temporal-frequency of visual motion. In the first experiment, adult participants observed static and moving gratings. The frequency of the MEG gamma response monotonically increased with motion velocity whereas power followed a bell-shape. In the second experiment, on a large group of children and adults, we found that despite drastic developmental changes in frequency and power of gamma oscillations, the relative suppression at high motion velocities was scaled to the same range of values across the life-span. In light of animal and modeling studies, the modulation of gamma power and frequency at high stimulation intensities characterizes the capacity of inhibitory neurons to counterbalance increasing excitation in visual networks. Gamma suppression may thus provide a non- invasive measure of inhibitory-based gain control in the healthy and diseased brain.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Gamma-band oscillations arise from the interplay between neural excitation (E) and inhibition (I) and may provide a non-invasive window into the state of cortical circuitry. A bell-shaped modulation of gamma response power by increasing the intensity of sensory input was observed in animals and is thought to reflect neural gain control. Here we sought to find a similar input-output relationship in humans with MEG via modulating the intensity of a visual stimulation by changing the velocity/ temporal-frequency of visual motion. In the first experiment, adult participants observed static and moving gratings. The frequency of the MEG gamma response monotonically increased with motion velocity whereas power followed a bell-shape. In the second experiment, on a large group of children and adults, we found that despite drastic developmental changes in frequency and power of gamma oscillations, the relative suppression at high motion velocities was scaled to the same range of values across the life-span. In light of animal and modeling studies, the modulation of gamma power and frequency at high stimulation intensities characterizes the capacity of inhibitory neurons to counterbalance increasing excitation in visual networks. Gamma suppression may thus provide a non- invasive measure of inhibitory-based gain control in the healthy and diseased brain.

Close

  • doi:10.1038/s41598-018-26779-6

Close

Hyojin Park; Robin A. A. Ince; Philippe G. Schyns; Gregor Thut; Joachim Gross

Representational interactions during audiovisual speech entrainment: Redundancy in left posterior superior temporal gyrus and synergy in left motor cortex Journal Article

In: PLoS Biology, vol. 16, no. 8, pp. e2006558, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Park2018,
title = {Representational interactions during audiovisual speech entrainment: Redundancy in left posterior superior temporal gyrus and synergy in left motor cortex},
author = {Hyojin Park and Robin A. A. Ince and Philippe G. Schyns and Gregor Thut and Joachim Gross},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pbio.2006558},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {PLoS Biology},
volume = {16},
number = {8},
pages = {e2006558},
abstract = {Integration of multimodal sensory information is fundamental to many aspects of human behavior, but the neural mechanisms underlying these processes remain mysterious. For example, during face-to-face communication, we know that the brain integrates dynamic auditory and visual inputs, but we do not yet understand where and how such integration mechanisms support speech comprehension. Here, we quantify representational interactions between dynamic audio and visual speech signals and show that different brain regions exhibit different types of representational interaction. With a novel information theoretic measure, we found that theta (3-7 Hz) oscillations in the posterior superior temporal gyrus/sulcus (pSTG/S) represent auditory and visual inputs redundantly (i.e., represent common features of the two), whereas the same oscillations in left motor and inferior temporal cortex represent the inputs synergistically (i.e., the instantaneous relationship between audio and visual inputs is also represented). Importantly, redundant coding in the left pSTG/S and synergistic coding in the left motor cortex predict behavior-i.e., speech comprehension performance. Our findings therefore demonstrate that processes classically described as integration can have different statistical properties and may reflect distinct mechanisms that occur in different brain regions to support audiovisual speech comprehension.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Integration of multimodal sensory information is fundamental to many aspects of human behavior, but the neural mechanisms underlying these processes remain mysterious. For example, during face-to-face communication, we know that the brain integrates dynamic auditory and visual inputs, but we do not yet understand where and how such integration mechanisms support speech comprehension. Here, we quantify representational interactions between dynamic audio and visual speech signals and show that different brain regions exhibit different types of representational interaction. With a novel information theoretic measure, we found that theta (3-7 Hz) oscillations in the posterior superior temporal gyrus/sulcus (pSTG/S) represent auditory and visual inputs redundantly (i.e., represent common features of the two), whereas the same oscillations in left motor and inferior temporal cortex represent the inputs synergistically (i.e., the instantaneous relationship between audio and visual inputs is also represented). Importantly, redundant coding in the left pSTG/S and synergistic coding in the left motor cortex predict behavior-i.e., speech comprehension performance. Our findings therefore demonstrate that processes classically described as integration can have different statistical properties and may reflect distinct mechanisms that occur in different brain regions to support audiovisual speech comprehension.

Close

  • doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.2006558

Close

Thomas Pfeffer; Arthur Ervin Avramiea; Guido Nolte; Andreas K. Engel; Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen; Tobias H. Donner

Catecholamines alter the intrinsic variability of cortical population activity and perception Journal Article

In: PLoS Biology, vol. 16, no. 2, pp. e2003453, 2018.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Pfeffer2018,
title = {Catecholamines alter the intrinsic variability of cortical population activity and perception},
author = {Thomas Pfeffer and Arthur Ervin Avramiea and Guido Nolte and Andreas K. Engel and Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen and Tobias H. Donner},
doi = {10.1371/journal.pbio.2003453},
year = {2018},
date = {2018-01-01},
journal = {PLoS Biology},
volume = {16},
number = {2},
pages = {e2003453},
abstract = {The ascending modulatory systems of the brain stem are powerful regulators of global brain state. Disturbances of these systems are implicated in several major neuropsychiatric disorders. Yet, how these systems interact with specific neural computations in the cerebral cortex to shape perception, cognition, and behavior remains poorly understood. Here, we probed into the effect of two such systems, the catecholaminergic (dopaminergic and noradrenergic) and cholinergic systems, on an important aspect of cortical computation: its intrinsic variability. To this end, we combined placebo-controlled pharmacological intervention in humans, recordings of cortical population activity using magnetoencephalography (MEG), and psychophysical measurements of the perception of ambiguous visual input. A low-dose catecholaminergic, but not cholinergic, manipulation altered the rate of spontaneous perceptual fluctuations as well as the temporal structure of “scale-free” population activity of large swaths of the visual and parietal cortices. Computational analyses indicate that both effects were consistent with an increase in excitatory relative to inhibitory activity in the cortical areas underlying visual perceptual inference. We propose that catecholamines regulate the variability of perception and cognition through dynamically changing the cortical excitation–inhibition ratio. The combined readout of fluctuations in perception and cortical activity we established here may prove useful as an efficient and easily accessible marker of altered cortical computation in neuropsychiatric disorders.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The ascending modulatory systems of the brain stem are powerful regulators of global brain state. Disturbances of these systems are implicated in several major neuropsychiatric disorders. Yet, how these systems interact with specific neural computations in the cerebral cortex to shape perception, cognition, and behavior remains poorly understood. Here, we probed into the effect of two such systems, the catecholaminergic (dopaminergic and noradrenergic) and cholinergic systems, on an important aspect of cortical computation: its intrinsic variability. To this end, we combined placebo-controlled pharmacological intervention in humans, recordings of cortical population activity using magnetoencephalography (MEG), and psychophysical measurements of the perception of ambiguous visual input. A low-dose catecholaminergic, but not cholinergic, manipulation altered the rate of spontaneous perceptual fluctuations as well as the temporal structure of “scale-free” population activity of large swaths of the visual and parietal cortices. Computational analyses indicate that both effects were consistent with an increase in excitatory relative to inhibitory activity in the cortical areas underlying visual perceptual inference. We propose that catecholamines regulate the variability of perception and cognition through dynamically changing the cortical excitation–inhibition ratio. The combined readout of fluctuations in perception and cortical activity we established here may prove useful as an efficient and easily accessible marker of altered cortical computation in neuropsychiatric disorders.

Close

  • doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.2003453

Close

2017

Grace Edwards; Petra Vetter; Fiona McGruer; Lucy S. Petro; Lars Muckli

Predictive feedback to V1 dynamically updates with sensory input Journal Article

In: Scientific Reports, vol. 7, pp. 16538, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Edwards2017a,
title = {Predictive feedback to V1 dynamically updates with sensory input},
author = {Grace Edwards and Petra Vetter and Fiona McGruer and Lucy S. Petro and Lars Muckli},
doi = {10.1038/s41598-017-16093-y},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-12-01},
journal = {Scientific Reports},
volume = {7},
pages = {16538},
publisher = {Springer US},
abstract = {Predictive coding theories propose that the brain creates internal models of the environment to predict upcoming sensory input. Hierarchical predictive coding models of vision postulate that higher visual areas generate predictions of sensory inputs and feed them back to early visual cortex. In V1, sensory inputs that do not match the predictions lead to amplified brain activation, but does this amplification process dynamically update to new retinotopic locations with eye-movements? We investigated the effect of eye-movements in predictive feedback using functional brain imaging and eye-tracking whilst presenting an apparent motion illusion. Apparent motion induces an internal model of motion, during which sensory predictions of the illusory motion feed back to V1. We observed attenuated BOLD responses to predicted stimuli at the new post-saccadic location in V1. Therefore, pre-saccadic predictions update their retinotopic location in time for post-saccadic input, validating dynamic predictive coding theories in V1.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Predictive coding theories propose that the brain creates internal models of the environment to predict upcoming sensory input. Hierarchical predictive coding models of vision postulate that higher visual areas generate predictions of sensory inputs and feed them back to early visual cortex. In V1, sensory inputs that do not match the predictions lead to amplified brain activation, but does this amplification process dynamically update to new retinotopic locations with eye-movements? We investigated the effect of eye-movements in predictive feedback using functional brain imaging and eye-tracking whilst presenting an apparent motion illusion. Apparent motion induces an internal model of motion, during which sensory predictions of the illusory motion feed back to V1. We observed attenuated BOLD responses to predicted stimuli at the new post-saccadic location in V1. Therefore, pre-saccadic predictions update their retinotopic location in time for post-saccadic input, validating dynamic predictive coding theories in V1.

Close

  • doi:10.1038/s41598-017-16093-y

Close

Carolyn McGettigan; Kyle Jasmin; Frank Eisner; Zarinah K. Agnew; Oliver J. Josephs; Andrew J. Calder; Rosemary Jessop; Rebecca P. Lawson; Mona Spielmann; Sophie K. Scott

You talkin' to me? Communicative talker gaze activates left-lateralized superior temporal cortex during perception of degraded speech Journal Article

In: Neuropsychologia, vol. 100, pp. 51–63, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{McGettigan2017,
title = {You talkin' to me? Communicative talker gaze activates left-lateralized superior temporal cortex during perception of degraded speech},
author = {Carolyn McGettigan and Kyle Jasmin and Frank Eisner and Zarinah K. Agnew and Oliver J. Josephs and Andrew J. Calder and Rosemary Jessop and Rebecca P. Lawson and Mona Spielmann and Sophie K. Scott},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.013},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-06-01},
journal = {Neuropsychologia},
volume = {100},
pages = {51--63},
publisher = {Pergamon},
abstract = {Neuroimaging studies of speech perception have consistently indicated a left-hemisphere dominance in the temporal lobes' responses to intelligible auditory speech signals (McGettigan and Scott, 2012). However, there are important communicative cues that cannot be extracted from auditory signals alone, including the direction of the talker's gaze. Previous work has implicated the superior temporal cortices in processing gaze direction, with evidence for predominantly right-lateralized responses (Carlin & Calder, 2013). The aim of the current study was to investigate whether the lateralization of responses to talker gaze differs in an auditory communicative context. Participants in a functional MRI experiment watched and listened to videos of spoken sentences in which the auditory intelligibility and talker gaze direction were manipulated factorially. We observed a left-dominant temporal lobe sensitivity to the talker's gaze direction, in which the left anterior superior temporal sulcus/gyrus and temporal pole showed an enhanced response to direct gaze – further investigation revealed that this pattern of lateralization was modulated by auditory intelligibility. Our results suggest flexibility in the distribution of neural responses to social cues in the face within the context of a challenging speech perception task.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Neuroimaging studies of speech perception have consistently indicated a left-hemisphere dominance in the temporal lobes' responses to intelligible auditory speech signals (McGettigan and Scott, 2012). However, there are important communicative cues that cannot be extracted from auditory signals alone, including the direction of the talker's gaze. Previous work has implicated the superior temporal cortices in processing gaze direction, with evidence for predominantly right-lateralized responses (Carlin & Calder, 2013). The aim of the current study was to investigate whether the lateralization of responses to talker gaze differs in an auditory communicative context. Participants in a functional MRI experiment watched and listened to videos of spoken sentences in which the auditory intelligibility and talker gaze direction were manipulated factorially. We observed a left-dominant temporal lobe sensitivity to the talker's gaze direction, in which the left anterior superior temporal sulcus/gyrus and temporal pole showed an enhanced response to direct gaze – further investigation revealed that this pattern of lateralization was modulated by auditory intelligibility. Our results suggest flexibility in the distribution of neural responses to social cues in the face within the context of a challenging speech perception task.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.013

Close

Evan M. Gordon; Timothy O. Laumann; Adrian W. Gilmore; Dillan J. Newbold; Deanna J. Greene; Jeffrey J. Berg; Mario Ortega; Catherine Hoyt-Drazen; Caterina Gratton; Haoxin Sun; Jacqueline M. Hampton; Rebecca S. Coalson; Annie L. Nguyen; Kathleen B. McDermott; Joshua S. Shimony; Abraham Z. Snyder; Bradley L. Schlaggar; Steven E. Petersen; Steven M. Nelson; Nico U. F. Dosenbach

Precision functional mapping of individual human brains Journal Article

In: Neuron, vol. 95, no. 4, pp. 791–807.e7, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Gordon2017,
title = {Precision functional mapping of individual human brains},
author = {Evan M. Gordon and Timothy O. Laumann and Adrian W. Gilmore and Dillan J. Newbold and Deanna J. Greene and Jeffrey J. Berg and Mario Ortega and Catherine Hoyt-Drazen and Caterina Gratton and Haoxin Sun and Jacqueline M. Hampton and Rebecca S. Coalson and Annie L. Nguyen and Kathleen B. McDermott and Joshua S. Shimony and Abraham Z. Snyder and Bradley L. Schlaggar and Steven E. Petersen and Steven M. Nelson and Nico U. F. Dosenbach},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.011},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Neuron},
volume = {95},
number = {4},
pages = {791--807.e7},
publisher = {Elsevier Inc.},
abstract = {Human functional MRI (fMRI) research primarily focuses on analyzing data averaged across groups, which limits the detail, specificity, and clinical utility of fMRI resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) and task-activation maps. To push our understanding of functional brain organization to the level of individual humans, we assembled a novel MRI dataset containing 5 hr of RSFC data, 6 hr of task fMRI, multiple structural MRIs, and neuropsychological tests from each of ten adults. Using these data, we generated ten high-fidelity, individual-specific functional connectomes. This individual-connectome approach revealed several new types of spatial and organizational variability in brain networks, including unique network features and topologies that corresponded with structural and task-derived brain features. We are releasing this highly sampled, individual-focused dataset as a resource for neuroscientists, and we propose precision individual connectomics as a model for future work examining the organization of healthy and diseased individual human brains.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Human functional MRI (fMRI) research primarily focuses on analyzing data averaged across groups, which limits the detail, specificity, and clinical utility of fMRI resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) and task-activation maps. To push our understanding of functional brain organization to the level of individual humans, we assembled a novel MRI dataset containing 5 hr of RSFC data, 6 hr of task fMRI, multiple structural MRIs, and neuropsychological tests from each of ten adults. Using these data, we generated ten high-fidelity, individual-specific functional connectomes. This individual-connectome approach revealed several new types of spatial and organizational variability in brain networks, including unique network features and topologies that corresponded with structural and task-derived brain features. We are releasing this highly sampled, individual-focused dataset as a resource for neuroscientists, and we propose precision individual connectomics as a model for future work examining the organization of healthy and diseased individual human brains.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.011

Close

Joseph C. Griffis; Abdurahman S. Elkhetali; Wesley K. Burge; Richard H. Chen; Anthony D. Bowman; Jerzy P. Szaflarski; Kristina M. Visscher

Retinotopic patterns of functional connectivity between V1 and large-scale brain networks during resting fixation Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 146, pp. 1071–1083, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Griffis2017,
title = {Retinotopic patterns of functional connectivity between V1 and large-scale brain networks during resting fixation},
author = {Joseph C. Griffis and Abdurahman S. Elkhetali and Wesley K. Burge and Richard H. Chen and Anthony D. Bowman and Jerzy P. Szaflarski and Kristina M. Visscher},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.035},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {146},
pages = {1071--1083},
abstract = {Psychophysical and neurobiological evidence suggests that central and peripheral vision are specialized for different functions. This specialization of function might be expected to lead to differences in the large-scale functional interactions of early cortical areas that represent central and peripheral visual space. Here, we characterize differences in whole-brain functional connectivity among sectors in primary visual cortex (V1) corresponding to central, near-peripheral, and far-peripheral vision during resting fixation. Importantly, our analyses reveal that eccentricity sectors in V1 have different functional connectivity with non-visual areas associated with large-scale brain networks. Regions associated with the fronto-parietal control network are most strongly connected with central sectors of V1, regions associated with the cingulo-opercular control network are most strongly connected with near-peripheral sectors of V1, and regions associated with the default mode and auditory networks are most strongly connected with far-peripheral sectors of V1. Additional analyses suggest that similar patterns are present during eyes-closed rest. These results suggest that different types of visual information may be prioritized by large-scale brain networks with distinct functional profiles, and provide insights into how the small-scale functional specialization within early visual regions such as V1 relates to the large-scale organization of functionally distinct whole-brain networks.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Psychophysical and neurobiological evidence suggests that central and peripheral vision are specialized for different functions. This specialization of function might be expected to lead to differences in the large-scale functional interactions of early cortical areas that represent central and peripheral visual space. Here, we characterize differences in whole-brain functional connectivity among sectors in primary visual cortex (V1) corresponding to central, near-peripheral, and far-peripheral vision during resting fixation. Importantly, our analyses reveal that eccentricity sectors in V1 have different functional connectivity with non-visual areas associated with large-scale brain networks. Regions associated with the fronto-parietal control network are most strongly connected with central sectors of V1, regions associated with the cingulo-opercular control network are most strongly connected with near-peripheral sectors of V1, and regions associated with the default mode and auditory networks are most strongly connected with far-peripheral sectors of V1. Additional analyses suggest that similar patterns are present during eyes-closed rest. These results suggest that different types of visual information may be prioritized by large-scale brain networks with distinct functional profiles, and provide insights into how the small-scale functional specialization within early visual regions such as V1 relates to the large-scale organization of functionally distinct whole-brain networks.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.035

Close

Erno J. Hermans; Jonathan W. Kanen; Arielle Tambini; Guillén Fernández; Lila Davachi; Elizabeth A. Phelps

Persistence of amygdala-hippocampal connectivity and multi-voxel correlation structures during awake rest after fear learning predicts long-term expression of fear Journal Article

In: Cerebral Cortex, vol. 27, no. 5, pp. 3028–3041, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Hermans2017,
title = {Persistence of amygdala-hippocampal connectivity and multi-voxel correlation structures during awake rest after fear learning predicts long-term expression of fear},
author = {Erno J. Hermans and Jonathan W. Kanen and Arielle Tambini and Guillén Fernández and Lila Davachi and Elizabeth A. Phelps},
doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhw145},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
volume = {27},
number = {5},
pages = {3028--3041},
abstract = {After encoding, memories undergo a process of consolidation that determines long-term retention. For conditioned fear, animal models postulate that consolidation involves reactivations of neuronal assemblies supporting fear learning during postlearning " offline " periods. However, no human studies to date have investigated such processes, particularly in relation to long-term expression of fear. We tested 24 participants using functional MRI on 2 consecutive days in a fear conditioning paradigm involving 1 habituation block, 2 acquisition blocks, and 2 extinction blocks on day 1, and 2 re-extinction blocks on day 2. Conditioning blocks were preceded and followed by 4.5-min rest blocks. Strength of spontaneous recovery of fear on day 2 served as a measure of long-term expression of fear. Amygdala connectivity primarily with hippocampus increased progressively during postacquisition and postextinction rest on day 1. Intraregional multi-voxel correlation structures within amygdala and hippocampus sampled during a block of differential fear conditioning furthermore persisted after fear learning. Critically, both these main findings were stronger in participants who exhibited spontaneous recovery 24 h later. Our findings indicate that neural circuits activated during fear conditioning exhibit persistent postlearning activity that may be functionally relevant in promoting consolidation of the fear memory.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

After encoding, memories undergo a process of consolidation that determines long-term retention. For conditioned fear, animal models postulate that consolidation involves reactivations of neuronal assemblies supporting fear learning during postlearning " offline " periods. However, no human studies to date have investigated such processes, particularly in relation to long-term expression of fear. We tested 24 participants using functional MRI on 2 consecutive days in a fear conditioning paradigm involving 1 habituation block, 2 acquisition blocks, and 2 extinction blocks on day 1, and 2 re-extinction blocks on day 2. Conditioning blocks were preceded and followed by 4.5-min rest blocks. Strength of spontaneous recovery of fear on day 2 served as a measure of long-term expression of fear. Amygdala connectivity primarily with hippocampus increased progressively during postacquisition and postextinction rest on day 1. Intraregional multi-voxel correlation structures within amygdala and hippocampus sampled during a block of differential fear conditioning furthermore persisted after fear learning. Critically, both these main findings were stronger in participants who exhibited spontaneous recovery 24 h later. Our findings indicate that neural circuits activated during fear conditioning exhibit persistent postlearning activity that may be functionally relevant in promoting consolidation of the fear memory.

Close

  • doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw145

Close

Jaakko Hotta; Jukka Saari; Miika Koskinen; Yevhen Hlushchuk; Nina Forss; Riitta Hari

Abnormal brain responses to action observation in complex regional pain syndrome Journal Article

In: Journal of Pain, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 255–265, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Hotta2017,
title = {Abnormal brain responses to action observation in complex regional pain syndrome},
author = {Jaakko Hotta and Jukka Saari and Miika Koskinen and Yevhen Hlushchuk and Nina Forss and Riitta Hari},
doi = {10.1016/j.jpain.2016.10.017},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Pain},
volume = {18},
number = {3},
pages = {255--265},
publisher = {Elsevier Inc},
abstract = {Patients with complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) display various abnormalities in central motor function, and their pain is intensified when they perform or just observe motor actions. In this study, we examined the abnormalities of brain responses to action observation in CRPS. We analyzed 3-T functional magnetic resonance images from 13 upper limb CRPS patients (all female, ages 31–58 years) and 13 healthy, age- and sex-matched control subjects. The functional magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired while the subjects viewed brief videos of hand actions shown in the first-person perspective. A pattern-classification analysis was applied to characterize brain areas where the activation pattern differed between CRPS patients and healthy subjects. Brain areas with statistically significant group differences (q < .05, false discovery rate-corrected) included the hand representation area in the sensorimotor cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, secondary somatosensory cortex, inferior parietal lobule, orbitofrontal cortex, and thalamus. Our findings indicate that CRPS impairs action observation by affecting brain areas related to pain processing and motor control. Perspective This article shows that in CRPS, the observation of others' motor actions induces abnormal neural activity in brain areas essential for sensorimotor functions and pain. These results build the cerebral basis for action-observation impairments in CRPS.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Patients with complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) display various abnormalities in central motor function, and their pain is intensified when they perform or just observe motor actions. In this study, we examined the abnormalities of brain responses to action observation in CRPS. We analyzed 3-T functional magnetic resonance images from 13 upper limb CRPS patients (all female, ages 31–58 years) and 13 healthy, age- and sex-matched control subjects. The functional magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired while the subjects viewed brief videos of hand actions shown in the first-person perspective. A pattern-classification analysis was applied to characterize brain areas where the activation pattern differed between CRPS patients and healthy subjects. Brain areas with statistically significant group differences (q < .05, false discovery rate-corrected) included the hand representation area in the sensorimotor cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, secondary somatosensory cortex, inferior parietal lobule, orbitofrontal cortex, and thalamus. Our findings indicate that CRPS impairs action observation by affecting brain areas related to pain processing and motor control. Perspective This article shows that in CRPS, the observation of others' motor actions induces abnormal neural activity in brain areas essential for sensorimotor functions and pain. These results build the cerebral basis for action-observation impairments in CRPS.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2016.10.017

Close

Su Keun Jeong; Yaoda Xu

Task-context-dependent linear representation of multiple visual objects in human parietal cortex Journal Article

In: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 29, no. 10, pp. 1778–1789, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Jeong2017,
title = {Task-context-dependent linear representation of multiple visual objects in human parietal cortex},
author = {Su Keun Jeong and Yaoda Xu},
doi = {10.1162/jocn_a_01156},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience},
volume = {29},
number = {10},
pages = {1778--1789},
abstract = {A host of recent studies have reported robust representations of visual object information in the human parietal cortex, similar to those found in ventral visual cortex. In ventral visual cortex, both monkey neurophysiology and human fMRI studies showed that the neural representation ofa pair ofunrelated objects can be approximated by the averaged neural representation of the constituent objects shown in isolation. In this study, we examined whether such a linear relationship between objects exists for object representations in the human parietal cortex. Using fMRI and multivoxel pattern analysis, we examined object representations in human inferior and superior intraparietal sulcus, two parietal regions previously implicated in visual object selection and encoding, respectively. We also examined responses from the lateral occipital region, a ventral object processing area. We obtained fMRI response patterns to object pairs and their constituent objects shown in isolation while participants viewed these objects and performed a 1-back repetition detection task. By measuring fMRI response pattern correlations, we found that all three brain regions contained representations for both single object and object pairs. In the lateral occipital region, the representation for a pair ofobjects could be reliably approximated by the average representation of its constituent objects shown in isolation, replicating previous findings in ventral visual cortex. Such a simple linear relationship, however, was not observed in either parietal region examined. Nevertheless, when we equated the amount of task information present by examining responses from two pairs of objects, we found that representations for the average of two object pairs were indistinguishable in both parietal regions from the average of another two object pairs containing the same four component objects but with a different pairing of the objects (i.e., the average of AB and CD vs. that of AD and CB). Thus, when task information was held consistent, the same linear relationship may govern how multiple independent objects are represented in the human parietal cortex as it does in ventral visual cortex. These findings show that object and task representations coexist in the human parietal cortex and characterize one significant dif- ference of how visual information may be represented in ventral visual and parietal regions.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

A host of recent studies have reported robust representations of visual object information in the human parietal cortex, similar to those found in ventral visual cortex. In ventral visual cortex, both monkey neurophysiology and human fMRI studies showed that the neural representation ofa pair ofunrelated objects can be approximated by the averaged neural representation of the constituent objects shown in isolation. In this study, we examined whether such a linear relationship between objects exists for object representations in the human parietal cortex. Using fMRI and multivoxel pattern analysis, we examined object representations in human inferior and superior intraparietal sulcus, two parietal regions previously implicated in visual object selection and encoding, respectively. We also examined responses from the lateral occipital region, a ventral object processing area. We obtained fMRI response patterns to object pairs and their constituent objects shown in isolation while participants viewed these objects and performed a 1-back repetition detection task. By measuring fMRI response pattern correlations, we found that all three brain regions contained representations for both single object and object pairs. In the lateral occipital region, the representation for a pair ofobjects could be reliably approximated by the average representation of its constituent objects shown in isolation, replicating previous findings in ventral visual cortex. Such a simple linear relationship, however, was not observed in either parietal region examined. Nevertheless, when we equated the amount of task information present by examining responses from two pairs of objects, we found that representations for the average of two object pairs were indistinguishable in both parietal regions from the average of another two object pairs containing the same four component objects but with a different pairing of the objects (i.e., the average of AB and CD vs. that of AD and CB). Thus, when task information was held consistent, the same linear relationship may govern how multiple independent objects are represented in the human parietal cortex as it does in ventral visual cortex. These findings show that object and task representations coexist in the human parietal cortex and characterize one significant dif- ference of how visual information may be represented in ventral visual and parietal regions.

Close

  • doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01156

Close

Anna B. Kuhns; Pascasie L. Dombert; Paola Mengotti; Gereon R. Fink; Simone Vossel

Spatial attention, motor intention, and Bayesian cue predictability in the human brain Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 37, no. 21, pp. 5334–5344, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Kuhns2017,
title = {Spatial attention, motor intention, and Bayesian cue predictability in the human brain},
author = {Anna B. Kuhns and Pascasie L. Dombert and Paola Mengotti and Gereon R. Fink and Simone Vossel},
doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3255-16.2017},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {37},
number = {21},
pages = {5334--5344},
abstract = {Predictions about upcoming events influence how we perceive and respond to our environment. There is increasing evidence that predictions may be generated based upon previous observations following Bayesian principles, but little is known about the underlying corticalmechanismsandtheir specificity for different cognitive subsystems.Thepresent studyaimedat identifyingcommonanddistinct neural signatures of predictive processing in the spatial attentional and motor intentional system. Twenty-three female and male healthy human volunteers performed two probabilistic cueing tasks with either spatial or motor cues while lying in the fMRI scanner. In these tasks, the percentage of cue validity changed unpredictably over time. Trialwise estimates of cue predictability were derived from a Bayesian observer model of behavioral responses. These estimates were included as parametric regressors for analyzing the BOLD time series. Parametric effects of cue predictability in valid and invalid trials were considered to reflect belief updating by precision-weighted prediction errors. The brain areas exhibiting predictability-dependent effects dissociated between the spatial attention and motor inten- tion task, with the right temporoparietal cortex being involved during spatial attention and the left angular gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex during motor intention. Connectivity analyses revealed that all three areas showed predictability-dependent coupling with the right hippocampus. These results suggest that precision-weighted prediction errors of stimulus locations and motor responses are encoded in distinct brain regions, but that crosstalk with the hippocampusmaybe necessary to integrate new trialwise outcomes in both cognitive systems.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Predictions about upcoming events influence how we perceive and respond to our environment. There is increasing evidence that predictions may be generated based upon previous observations following Bayesian principles, but little is known about the underlying corticalmechanismsandtheir specificity for different cognitive subsystems.Thepresent studyaimedat identifyingcommonanddistinct neural signatures of predictive processing in the spatial attentional and motor intentional system. Twenty-three female and male healthy human volunteers performed two probabilistic cueing tasks with either spatial or motor cues while lying in the fMRI scanner. In these tasks, the percentage of cue validity changed unpredictably over time. Trialwise estimates of cue predictability were derived from a Bayesian observer model of behavioral responses. These estimates were included as parametric regressors for analyzing the BOLD time series. Parametric effects of cue predictability in valid and invalid trials were considered to reflect belief updating by precision-weighted prediction errors. The brain areas exhibiting predictability-dependent effects dissociated between the spatial attention and motor inten- tion task, with the right temporoparietal cortex being involved during spatial attention and the left angular gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex during motor intention. Connectivity analyses revealed that all three areas showed predictability-dependent coupling with the right hippocampus. These results suggest that precision-weighted prediction errors of stimulus locations and motor responses are encoded in distinct brain regions, but that crosstalk with the hippocampusmaybe necessary to integrate new trialwise outcomes in both cognitive systems.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3255-16.2017

Close

Jeongmi Lee; Joy J. Geng

Idiosyncratic patterns of representational similarity in prefrontal cortex predict attentional performance Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 37, no. 5, pp. 1257–1268, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Lee2017a,
title = {Idiosyncratic patterns of representational similarity in prefrontal cortex predict attentional performance},
author = {Jeongmi Lee and Joy J. Geng},
doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1407-16.2016},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {37},
number = {5},
pages = {1257--1268},
abstract = {The efficiency of finding an object in a crowded environment depends largely on the similarity of nontargets to the search target. Models of attention theorize that the similarity is determined by representations stored within an "attentional template" held in working memory. However, the degree to which the contents of the attentional template are individually unique and where those idiosyncratic representations are encoded in the brain are unknown. We investigated this problem using representational similarity analysis of human fMRI data to measure the common and idiosyncratic representations of famous face morphs during an identity categorization task; data from the categorization task were then used to predict performance on a separate identity search task. We hypothesized that the idiosyncratic categorical representations of the continuous face morphs would predict their distractability when searching for each target identity. The results identified that patterns of activation in the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) as well as in face-selective areas in the ventral temporal cortex were highly correlated with the patterns of behavioral categorization of face morphs and search performance that were common across subjects. However, the individually unique components of the categorization behavior were reliably decoded only in right LPFC. Moreover, the neural pattern in right LPFC successfully predicted idiosyncratic variability in search performance, such that reaction times were longer when distractors had a higher probability of being categorized as the target identity. These results suggest that the prefrontal cortex encodes individually unique components of categorical representations that are also present in attentional tem-plates for target search.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The efficiency of finding an object in a crowded environment depends largely on the similarity of nontargets to the search target. Models of attention theorize that the similarity is determined by representations stored within an "attentional template" held in working memory. However, the degree to which the contents of the attentional template are individually unique and where those idiosyncratic representations are encoded in the brain are unknown. We investigated this problem using representational similarity analysis of human fMRI data to measure the common and idiosyncratic representations of famous face morphs during an identity categorization task; data from the categorization task were then used to predict performance on a separate identity search task. We hypothesized that the idiosyncratic categorical representations of the continuous face morphs would predict their distractability when searching for each target identity. The results identified that patterns of activation in the lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) as well as in face-selective areas in the ventral temporal cortex were highly correlated with the patterns of behavioral categorization of face morphs and search performance that were common across subjects. However, the individually unique components of the categorization behavior were reliably decoded only in right LPFC. Moreover, the neural pattern in right LPFC successfully predicted idiosyncratic variability in search performance, such that reaction times were longer when distractors had a higher probability of being categorized as the target identity. These results suggest that the prefrontal cortex encodes individually unique components of categorical representations that are also present in attentional tem-plates for target search.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1407-16.2016

Close

Laura Leuchs; Max Schneider; Michael Czisch; Victor I. Spoormaker

Neural correlates of pupil dilation during human fear learning Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 147, pp. 186–197, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Leuchs2017,
title = {Neural correlates of pupil dilation during human fear learning},
author = {Laura Leuchs and Max Schneider and Michael Czisch and Victor I. Spoormaker},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.072},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {147},
pages = {186--197},
publisher = {Elsevier},
abstract = {Background: Fear conditioning and extinction are prevailing experimental and etiological models for normal and pathological anxiety. Pupil dilations in response to conditioned stimuli are increasingly used as a robust psychophysiological readout of fear learning, but their neural correlates remain unknown. We aimed at identifying the neural correlates of pupil responses to threat and safety cues during a fear learning task. Methods: Thirty-four healthy subjects underwent a fear conditioning and extinction paradigm with simultaneous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and pupillometry. After a stringent preprocessing and artifact rejection procedure, trial-wise pupil responses to threat and safety cues were entered as parametric modulations to the fMRI general linear models. Results: Trial-wise magnitude of pupil responses to both conditioned and safety stimuli correlated positively with activity in dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), thalamus, supramarginal gyrus and insula for the entire fear learning task, and with activity in the dACC during the fear conditioning phase in particular. Phasic pupil responses did not show habituation, but were negatively correlated with tonic baseline pupil diameter, which decreased during the task. Correcting phasic pupil responses for the tonic baseline pupil diameter revealed thalamic activity, which was also observed in an analysis employing a linear (declining) time modulation. Conclusion: Pupil dilations during fear conditioning and extinction provide useful readouts to track fear learning on a trial-by-trial level, particularly with simultaneous fMRI. Whereas phasic pupil responses reflect activity in brain regions involved in fear learning and threat appraisal, most prominently in dACC, tonic changes in pupil diameter may reflect changes in general arousal.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Background: Fear conditioning and extinction are prevailing experimental and etiological models for normal and pathological anxiety. Pupil dilations in response to conditioned stimuli are increasingly used as a robust psychophysiological readout of fear learning, but their neural correlates remain unknown. We aimed at identifying the neural correlates of pupil responses to threat and safety cues during a fear learning task. Methods: Thirty-four healthy subjects underwent a fear conditioning and extinction paradigm with simultaneous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and pupillometry. After a stringent preprocessing and artifact rejection procedure, trial-wise pupil responses to threat and safety cues were entered as parametric modulations to the fMRI general linear models. Results: Trial-wise magnitude of pupil responses to both conditioned and safety stimuli correlated positively with activity in dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), thalamus, supramarginal gyrus and insula for the entire fear learning task, and with activity in the dACC during the fear conditioning phase in particular. Phasic pupil responses did not show habituation, but were negatively correlated with tonic baseline pupil diameter, which decreased during the task. Correcting phasic pupil responses for the tonic baseline pupil diameter revealed thalamic activity, which was also observed in an analysis employing a linear (declining) time modulation. Conclusion: Pupil dilations during fear conditioning and extinction provide useful readouts to track fear learning on a trial-by-trial level, particularly with simultaneous fMRI. Whereas phasic pupil responses reflect activity in brain regions involved in fear learning and threat appraisal, most prominently in dACC, tonic changes in pupil diameter may reflect changes in general arousal.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.072

Close

Liu D. Liu; Christopher C. Pack

The contribution of area MT to visual motion perception depends on training Journal Article

In: Neuron, vol. 95, no. 2, pp. 436–446.e3, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Liu2017,
title = {The contribution of area MT to visual motion perception depends on training},
author = {Liu D. Liu and Christopher C. Pack},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.024},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Neuron},
volume = {95},
number = {2},
pages = {436--446.e3},
publisher = {Elsevier Inc.},
abstract = {Perceptual decisions require the transformation of raw sensory inputs into cortical representations suitable for stimulus discrimination. One of the best-known examples of this transformation involves the middle temporal area (MT) of the primate visual cortex. Area MT provides a robust representation of stimulus motion, and previous work has shown that it contributes causally to performance on motion discrimination tasks. Here we report that the strength of this contribution can be highly plastic: depending on the recent training history, pharmacological inactivation of MT can severely impair motion discrimination, or it can have little detectable influence. Further analysis of neural and behavioral data suggests that training moves the readout of motion information between MT and lower-level cortical areas. These results show that the contribution of individual brain regions to conscious perception can shift flexibly depending on sensory experience.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Perceptual decisions require the transformation of raw sensory inputs into cortical representations suitable for stimulus discrimination. One of the best-known examples of this transformation involves the middle temporal area (MT) of the primate visual cortex. Area MT provides a robust representation of stimulus motion, and previous work has shown that it contributes causally to performance on motion discrimination tasks. Here we report that the strength of this contribution can be highly plastic: depending on the recent training history, pharmacological inactivation of MT can severely impair motion discrimination, or it can have little detectable influence. Further analysis of neural and behavioral data suggests that training moves the readout of motion information between MT and lower-level cortical areas. These results show that the contribution of individual brain regions to conscious perception can shift flexibly depending on sensory experience.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.024

Close

Zhong-Xu Liu; Kelly Shen; Rosanna K. Olsen; Jennifer D. Ryan

Visual sampling predicts hippocampal activity Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 37, no. 3, pp. 599–609, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Liu2017c,
title = {Visual sampling predicts hippocampal activity},
author = {Zhong-Xu Liu and Kelly Shen and Rosanna K. Olsen and Jennifer D. Ryan},
doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2610-16.2017},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {37},
number = {3},
pages = {599--609},
abstract = {Eye movements serve to accumulate information from the visual world, contributing to the formation of coherent memory representations that support cognition and behavior. The hippocampus and the oculomotor network are well connected anatomically through an extensive set of polysynaptic pathways. However, the extent to which visual sampling behavior is related to functional responses in the hippocampus during encoding has not been studied directly in human neuroimaging. In the current study, participants engaged in a face processing task while brain responses were recorded with fMRI and eye movements were monitored simultaneously. The number of gaze fixations that a participant made on a given trial was correlated significantly with hippocampal activation such that more fixations were associated with stronger hippocampal activation. Similar results were also found in the fusiform face area, a face-selective perceptual processing region. Notably, the number of fixations was associated with stronger hippocampal activation when the presented faces were novel, but not when the faces were repeated. Increases in fixations during viewing of novel faces also led to larger repetition-related suppression in the hippocampus, indicating that this fixation–hippocampal relationship may reflect the ongoing development of lasting representations. Together, these results provide novel empirical support for the idea that visual exploration and hippocampal binding processes are inherently linked.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Eye movements serve to accumulate information from the visual world, contributing to the formation of coherent memory representations that support cognition and behavior. The hippocampus and the oculomotor network are well connected anatomically through an extensive set of polysynaptic pathways. However, the extent to which visual sampling behavior is related to functional responses in the hippocampus during encoding has not been studied directly in human neuroimaging. In the current study, participants engaged in a face processing task while brain responses were recorded with fMRI and eye movements were monitored simultaneously. The number of gaze fixations that a participant made on a given trial was correlated significantly with hippocampal activation such that more fixations were associated with stronger hippocampal activation. Similar results were also found in the fusiform face area, a face-selective perceptual processing region. Notably, the number of fixations was associated with stronger hippocampal activation when the presented faces were novel, but not when the faces were repeated. Increases in fixations during viewing of novel faces also led to larger repetition-related suppression in the hippocampus, indicating that this fixation–hippocampal relationship may reflect the ongoing development of lasting representations. Together, these results provide novel empirical support for the idea that visual exploration and hippocampal binding processes are inherently linked.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2610-16.2017

Close

Christopher R. Madan; Esther Fujiwara; Jeremy B. Caplan; Tobias Sommer

Emotional arousal impairs association-memory: Roles of amygdala and hippocampus Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 156, pp. 14–28, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Madan2017,
title = {Emotional arousal impairs association-memory: Roles of amygdala and hippocampus},
author = {Christopher R. Madan and Esther Fujiwara and Jeremy B. Caplan and Tobias Sommer},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.065},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {156},
pages = {14--28},
publisher = {Elsevier},
abstract = {Emotional arousal is well-known to enhance memory for individual items or events, whereas it can impair association memory. The neural mechanism of this association memory impairment by emotion is not known: In response to emotionally arousing information, amygdala activity may interfere with hippocampal associative encoding (e.g., via prefrontal cortex). Alternatively, emotional information may be harder to unitize, resulting in reduced availability of extra-hippocampal medial temporal lobe support for emotional than neutral associations. To test these opposing hypotheses, we compared neural processes underlying successful and unsuccessful encoding of emotional and neutral associations. Participants intentionally studied pairs of neutral and negative pictures (Experiments 1–3). We found reduced association-memory for negative pictures in all experiments, accompanied by item-memory increases in Experiment 2. High-resolution fMRI (Experiment 3) indicated that reductions in associative encoding of emotional information are localizable to an area in ventral-lateral amygdala, driven by attentional/salience effects in the central amygdala. Hippocampal activity was similar during both pair types, but a left hippocampal cluster related to successful encoding was observed only for negative pairs. Extra-hippocampal associative memory processes (e.g., unitization) were more effective for neutral than emotional materials. Our findings suggest that reduced emotional association memory is accompanied by increases in activity and functional coupling within the amygdala. This did not disrupt hippocampal association-memory processes, which indeed were critical for successful emotional association memory formation.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Emotional arousal is well-known to enhance memory for individual items or events, whereas it can impair association memory. The neural mechanism of this association memory impairment by emotion is not known: In response to emotionally arousing information, amygdala activity may interfere with hippocampal associative encoding (e.g., via prefrontal cortex). Alternatively, emotional information may be harder to unitize, resulting in reduced availability of extra-hippocampal medial temporal lobe support for emotional than neutral associations. To test these opposing hypotheses, we compared neural processes underlying successful and unsuccessful encoding of emotional and neutral associations. Participants intentionally studied pairs of neutral and negative pictures (Experiments 1–3). We found reduced association-memory for negative pictures in all experiments, accompanied by item-memory increases in Experiment 2. High-resolution fMRI (Experiment 3) indicated that reductions in associative encoding of emotional information are localizable to an area in ventral-lateral amygdala, driven by attentional/salience effects in the central amygdala. Hippocampal activity was similar during both pair types, but a left hippocampal cluster related to successful encoding was observed only for negative pairs. Extra-hippocampal associative memory processes (e.g., unitization) were more effective for neutral than emotional materials. Our findings suggest that reduced emotional association memory is accompanied by increases in activity and functional coupling within the amygdala. This did not disrupt hippocampal association-memory processes, which indeed were critical for successful emotional association memory formation.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.065

Close

Olivia M. Maynard; Jonathan C. W. Brooks; Marcus R. Munafò; Ute Leonards

Neural mechanisms underlying visual attention to health warnings on branded and plain cigarette packs Journal Article

In: Addiction, vol. 112, no. 4, pp. 662–672, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Maynard2017,
title = {Neural mechanisms underlying visual attention to health warnings on branded and plain cigarette packs},
author = {Olivia M. Maynard and Jonathan C. W. Brooks and Marcus R. Munafò and Ute Leonards},
doi = {10.1111/add.13699},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Addiction},
volume = {112},
number = {4},
pages = {662--672},
abstract = {Aims: To (1) test if activation in brain regions related to reward (nucleus accumbens) and emotion (amygdala) differ when branded and plain packs of cigarettes are viewed, (2) test whether these activation patterns differ by smoking status and (3) examine whether activation patterns differ as a function of visual attention to health warning labels on cigarette packs. Design: Cross-sectional observational study combining functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with eye-tracking. Non-smokers, weekly smokers and daily smokers performed a memory task on branded and plain cigarette packs with pictorial health warnings presented in an event-related design. Setting: Clinical Research and Imaging Centre, University of Bristol, UK. Participants: Non-smokers, weekly smokers and daily smokers (n = 72) were tested. After exclusions, data from 19 non-smokers, 19 weekly smokers and 20 daily smokers were analysed. Measurements: Brain activity was assessed in whole brain analyses and in pre-specified masked analyses in the amygdala and nucleus accumbens. On-line eye-tracking during scanning recorded visual attention to health warnings. Findings: There was no evidence for a main effect of pack type or smoking status in either the nucleus accumbens or amygdala, and this was unchanged when taking account of visual attention to health warnings. However, there was evidence for an interaction, such that we observed increased activation in the right amygdala when viewing branded as compared with plain packs among weekly smokers (P = 0.003). When taking into account visual attention to health warnings, we observed higher levels of activation in the visual cortex in response to plain packaging compared with branded packaging of cigarettes (P = 0.020). Conclusions: Based on functional magnetic resonance imaging and eye-tracking data, health warnings appear to be more salient on ‘plain' cigarette packs than branded packs.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Aims: To (1) test if activation in brain regions related to reward (nucleus accumbens) and emotion (amygdala) differ when branded and plain packs of cigarettes are viewed, (2) test whether these activation patterns differ by smoking status and (3) examine whether activation patterns differ as a function of visual attention to health warning labels on cigarette packs. Design: Cross-sectional observational study combining functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with eye-tracking. Non-smokers, weekly smokers and daily smokers performed a memory task on branded and plain cigarette packs with pictorial health warnings presented in an event-related design. Setting: Clinical Research and Imaging Centre, University of Bristol, UK. Participants: Non-smokers, weekly smokers and daily smokers (n = 72) were tested. After exclusions, data from 19 non-smokers, 19 weekly smokers and 20 daily smokers were analysed. Measurements: Brain activity was assessed in whole brain analyses and in pre-specified masked analyses in the amygdala and nucleus accumbens. On-line eye-tracking during scanning recorded visual attention to health warnings. Findings: There was no evidence for a main effect of pack type or smoking status in either the nucleus accumbens or amygdala, and this was unchanged when taking account of visual attention to health warnings. However, there was evidence for an interaction, such that we observed increased activation in the right amygdala when viewing branded as compared with plain packs among weekly smokers (P = 0.003). When taking into account visual attention to health warnings, we observed higher levels of activation in the visual cortex in response to plain packaging compared with branded packaging of cigarettes (P = 0.020). Conclusions: Based on functional magnetic resonance imaging and eye-tracking data, health warnings appear to be more salient on ‘plain' cigarette packs than branded packs.

Close

  • doi:10.1111/add.13699

Close

Yuki Motomura; Ruri Katsunuma; Michitaka Yoshimura; Kazuo Mishima

Two days' sleep debt causes mood decline during resting state via diminished amygdala-prefrontal connectivity Journal Article

In: Sleep, vol. 40, no. 10, pp. zsx133, 2017.

Abstract | BibTeX

@article{Motomura2017,
title = {Two days' sleep debt causes mood decline during resting state via diminished amygdala-prefrontal connectivity},
author = {Yuki Motomura and Ruri Katsunuma and Michitaka Yoshimura and Kazuo Mishima},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Sleep},
volume = {40},
number = {10},
pages = {zsx133},
abstract = {Study objectives: Sleep debt (SD) has been suggested to evoke emotional instability by diminishing the suppression of the amygdala by the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC). Here, we investigated how short-term SD affects resting-state functional connectivity between the amygdala and MPFC, self-reported mood, and sleep parameters. Methods: Eighteen healthy adult men aged 29 ± 8.24 years participated in a 2-day sleep control session (SC; time in bed [TIB], 9 hours) and 2-day SD session (TIB, 3 hours). On day 2 of each session, resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging was performed, followed immediately by measuring self-reported mood on the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-State subscale (STAI-S). Results: STAI-S score was significantly increased, and functional connectivity between the amygdala and MPFC was significantly decreased in SD compared with SC. Significant correlations were observed between reduced rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and reduced left amygdala-MPFC functional connectivity (FCL_amg-MPFC ) and between reduced FCL_amg-MPFC and increased STAI-S score in SD compared with SC. Conclusions: These findings suggest that reduced MPFC functional connectivity of amygdala activity is involved in mood deterioration under SD, and that REM sleep reduction is involved in functional changes in the corresponding brain regions. Having adequate REM sleep may be important for mental health maintenance.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Study objectives: Sleep debt (SD) has been suggested to evoke emotional instability by diminishing the suppression of the amygdala by the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC). Here, we investigated how short-term SD affects resting-state functional connectivity between the amygdala and MPFC, self-reported mood, and sleep parameters. Methods: Eighteen healthy adult men aged 29 ± 8.24 years participated in a 2-day sleep control session (SC; time in bed [TIB], 9 hours) and 2-day SD session (TIB, 3 hours). On day 2 of each session, resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging was performed, followed immediately by measuring self-reported mood on the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-State subscale (STAI-S). Results: STAI-S score was significantly increased, and functional connectivity between the amygdala and MPFC was significantly decreased in SD compared with SC. Significant correlations were observed between reduced rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and reduced left amygdala-MPFC functional connectivity (FCL_amg-MPFC ) and between reduced FCL_amg-MPFC and increased STAI-S score in SD compared with SC. Conclusions: These findings suggest that reduced MPFC functional connectivity of amygdala activity is involved in mood deterioration under SD, and that REM sleep reduction is involved in functional changes in the corresponding brain regions. Having adequate REM sleep may be important for mental health maintenance.

Close

Alex L. White; Erik Runeson; John Palmer; Zachary R. Ernst; Geoffrey M. Boynton

Evidence for unlimited capacity processing of simple features in visual cortex Journal Article

In: Journal of Vision, vol. 17, no. 6, pp. 19, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{White2017a,
title = {Evidence for unlimited capacity processing of simple features in visual cortex},
author = {Alex L. White and Erik Runeson and John Palmer and Zachary R. Ernst and Geoffrey M. Boynton},
doi = {10.1167/17.6.19},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Vision},
volume = {17},
number = {6},
pages = {19},
abstract = {Performance in many visual tasks is impaired when observers attempt to divide spatial attention across multiple visual field locations. Correspondingly, neuronal response magnitudes in visual cortex are often reduced during divided compared with focused spatial attention. This suggests that early visual cortex is the site of capacity limits, where finite processing resources must be divided among attended stimuli. However, behavioral research demonstrates that not all visual tasks suffer such capacity limits: The costs of divided attention are minimal when the task and stimulus are simple, such as when searching for a target defined by orientation or contrast. To date, however, every neuroimaging study of divided attention has used more complex tasks and found large reductions in response magnitude. We bridged that gap by using functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure responses in the human visual cortex during simple feature detection. The first experiment used a visual search task: Observers detected a low-contrast Gabor patch within one or four potentially relevant locations. The second experiment used a dual-task design, in which observers made independent judgments of Gabor presence in patches of dynamic noise at two locations. In both experiments, blood-oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signals in the retinotopic cortex were significantly lower for ignored than attended stimuli. However, when observers divided attention between multiple stimuli, BOLD signals were not reliably reduced and behavioral performance was unimpaired. These results suggest that processing of simple features in early visual cortex has unlimited capacity.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Performance in many visual tasks is impaired when observers attempt to divide spatial attention across multiple visual field locations. Correspondingly, neuronal response magnitudes in visual cortex are often reduced during divided compared with focused spatial attention. This suggests that early visual cortex is the site of capacity limits, where finite processing resources must be divided among attended stimuli. However, behavioral research demonstrates that not all visual tasks suffer such capacity limits: The costs of divided attention are minimal when the task and stimulus are simple, such as when searching for a target defined by orientation or contrast. To date, however, every neuroimaging study of divided attention has used more complex tasks and found large reductions in response magnitude. We bridged that gap by using functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure responses in the human visual cortex during simple feature detection. The first experiment used a visual search task: Observers detected a low-contrast Gabor patch within one or four potentially relevant locations. The second experiment used a dual-task design, in which observers made independent judgments of Gabor presence in patches of dynamic noise at two locations. In both experiments, blood-oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signals in the retinotopic cortex were significantly lower for ignored than attended stimuli. However, when observers divided attention between multiple stimuli, BOLD signals were not reliably reduced and behavioral performance was unimpaired. These results suggest that processing of simple features in early visual cortex has unlimited capacity.

Close

  • doi:10.1167/17.6.19

Close

Kitty Z. Xu; Brian A. Anderson; Erik E. Emeric; Anthony W. Sali; Veit Stuphorn; Steven Yantis; Susan M. Courtney

Neural basis of cognitive control over movement inhibition: Human fMRI and primate electrophysiology evidence Journal Article

In: Neuron, vol. 96, no. 6, pp. 1447–1458.e6, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Xu2017,
title = {Neural basis of cognitive control over movement inhibition: Human fMRI and primate electrophysiology evidence},
author = {Kitty Z. Xu and Brian A. Anderson and Erik E. Emeric and Anthony W. Sali and Veit Stuphorn and Steven Yantis and Susan M. Courtney},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuron.2017.11.010},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Neuron},
volume = {96},
number = {6},
pages = {1447--1458.e6},
publisher = {Elsevier Inc.},
abstract = {Executive control involves the ability to flexibly inhibit or change an action when it is contextually inappropriate. Using the complimentary techniques of human fMRI and monkey electrophysiology in a context-dependent stop signal task, we found a functional double dissociation between the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (rVLPFC) and the bi-lateral frontal eye field (FEF). Different regions of rVLPFC were associated with context-based signal meaning versus intention to inhibit a response, while FEF activity corresponded to success or failure of the response inhibition regardless of the stimulus response mapping or the context. These results were validated by electrophysiological recordings in rVLPFC and FEF from one monkey. Inhibition of a planned behavior is therefore likely not governed by a single brain system as had been previously proposed, but instead depends on two distinct neural processes involving different sub-regions of the rVLPFC and their interactions with other motor-related brain regions. Xu et al. present a rare combination of complementary evidence from human fMRI and primate neurophysiology, demonstrating that response inhibition is not directly accomplished by the rVLPFC, but instead requires multiple, distinct rVLPFC networks involving attention and contextual stimulus interpretation.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Executive control involves the ability to flexibly inhibit or change an action when it is contextually inappropriate. Using the complimentary techniques of human fMRI and monkey electrophysiology in a context-dependent stop signal task, we found a functional double dissociation between the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (rVLPFC) and the bi-lateral frontal eye field (FEF). Different regions of rVLPFC were associated with context-based signal meaning versus intention to inhibit a response, while FEF activity corresponded to success or failure of the response inhibition regardless of the stimulus response mapping or the context. These results were validated by electrophysiological recordings in rVLPFC and FEF from one monkey. Inhibition of a planned behavior is therefore likely not governed by a single brain system as had been previously proposed, but instead depends on two distinct neural processes involving different sub-regions of the rVLPFC and their interactions with other motor-related brain regions. Xu et al. present a rare combination of complementary evidence from human fMRI and primate neurophysiology, demonstrating that response inhibition is not directly accomplished by the rVLPFC, but instead requires multiple, distinct rVLPFC networks involving attention and contextual stimulus interpretation.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2017.11.010

Close

C. J. Aine; H. J. Bockholt; J. R. Bustillo; J. M. Cañive; A. Caprihan; C. Gasparovic; F. M. Hanlon; J. M. Houck; R. E. Jung; J. Lauriello; J. Liu; A. R. Mayer; N. I. Perrone-Bizzozero; S. Posse; Julia M. Stephen; J. A. Turner; V. P. Clark; Vince D. Calhoun

Multimodal neuroimaging in schizophrenia: Description and dissemination Journal Article

In: Neuroinformatics, vol. 15, no. 4, pp. 343–364, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Aine2017,
title = {Multimodal neuroimaging in schizophrenia: Description and dissemination},
author = {C. J. Aine and H. J. Bockholt and J. R. Bustillo and J. M. Cañive and A. Caprihan and C. Gasparovic and F. M. Hanlon and J. M. Houck and R. E. Jung and J. Lauriello and J. Liu and A. R. Mayer and N. I. Perrone-Bizzozero and S. Posse and Julia M. Stephen and J. A. Turner and V. P. Clark and Vince D. Calhoun},
doi = {10.1007/s12021-017-9338-9},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Neuroinformatics},
volume = {15},
number = {4},
pages = {343--364},
publisher = {Neuroinformatics},
abstract = {In this paper we describe an open-access collection ofmultimodal neuroimaging data in schizophrenia for release to the community. Data were acquired from approximately 100 patients with schizophrenia and 100 age-matched controls during rest as well as several task activation paradigms targeting a hierarchy of cognitive constructs. Neuroimaging data include structural MRI, functional MRI, diffusion MRI, MR spectroscopic imaging, and magnetoencephalography. For three of the hypothesis-driven projects, task activation paradigms were acquired on subsets of$sim$200 volunteers which examined a range of sensory and cognitive processes (e.g., auditory sensory gating, auditory/visual multisensory integration, visual transverse patterning). Neuropsychological data were also acquired and genetic material via saliva samples were collected from most of the participants and have been typed for both genome-wide polymorphism data as well as genome-wide methylation data. Some results are also present- ed from the individual studies as well as from our data-driven multimodal analyses (e.g., multimodal examinations of network structure and network dynamics and multitask fMRI data analysis across projects). All data will be released through the Mind Research Network's collaborative informatics and neuroimaging suite (COINS).},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

In this paper we describe an open-access collection ofmultimodal neuroimaging data in schizophrenia for release to the community. Data were acquired from approximately 100 patients with schizophrenia and 100 age-matched controls during rest as well as several task activation paradigms targeting a hierarchy of cognitive constructs. Neuroimaging data include structural MRI, functional MRI, diffusion MRI, MR spectroscopic imaging, and magnetoencephalography. For three of the hypothesis-driven projects, task activation paradigms were acquired on subsets of$sim$200 volunteers which examined a range of sensory and cognitive processes (e.g., auditory sensory gating, auditory/visual multisensory integration, visual transverse patterning). Neuropsychological data were also acquired and genetic material via saliva samples were collected from most of the participants and have been typed for both genome-wide polymorphism data as well as genome-wide methylation data. Some results are also present- ed from the individual studies as well as from our data-driven multimodal analyses (e.g., multimodal examinations of network structure and network dynamics and multitask fMRI data analysis across projects). All data will be released through the Mind Research Network's collaborative informatics and neuroimaging suite (COINS).

Close

  • doi:10.1007/s12021-017-9338-9

Close

Thomas Meindertsma; Niels A. Kloosterman; Guido Nolte; Andreas K. Engel; Tobias H. Donner

Multiple transient signals in human visual cortex associated with an elementary decision Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 37, no. 23, pp. 5744–5757, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Meindertsma2017,
title = {Multiple transient signals in human visual cortex associated with an elementary decision},
author = {Thomas Meindertsma and Niels A. Kloosterman and Guido Nolte and Andreas K. Engel and Tobias H. Donner},
doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3835-16.2017},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {37},
number = {23},
pages = {5744--5757},
abstract = {The cerebral cortex continuously undergoes changes in its state, which are manifested in transient modulations of the cortical power spectrum. Cortical state changes also occur at full wakefulness and during rapid cognitive acts, such as perceptual decisions. Previous studies found a global modulation of beta-band (12–30 Hz) activity in human and monkey visual cortex during an elementary visual decision: reporting the appearance or disappearance of salient visual targets surrounded by a distractor. The previous studies disentangled neither the motor action associated with behavioral report nor other secondary processes, such as arousal, from perceptual decision processing per se. Here, we used magnetoencephalography in humans to pinpoint the factors underlying the beta-band modulation.We found that disappearances of a salient target were associated with beta-band suppression, and target reappearances with beta-band enhancement. This was true for both overt behavioral reports (immediate button presses) and silent counting of the perceptual events. This finding indicates that the beta-band modulation was unrelated to the execution of the motor act associated with a behavioral report of the perceptual decision. Further, changes in pupil-linked arousal, fixational eye movements, or gamma-band responses were not necessary for the beta-band modulation. Together, our results suggest that the beta-band modulation was a top-down signal associated with the process of converting graded perceptual signals into a categorical format underlying flexible behavior. This signal may have been fed back from brain regions involved in decision processing to visual cortex, thus enforcing a “decision-consistent” cortical state.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The cerebral cortex continuously undergoes changes in its state, which are manifested in transient modulations of the cortical power spectrum. Cortical state changes also occur at full wakefulness and during rapid cognitive acts, such as perceptual decisions. Previous studies found a global modulation of beta-band (12–30 Hz) activity in human and monkey visual cortex during an elementary visual decision: reporting the appearance or disappearance of salient visual targets surrounded by a distractor. The previous studies disentangled neither the motor action associated with behavioral report nor other secondary processes, such as arousal, from perceptual decision processing per se. Here, we used magnetoencephalography in humans to pinpoint the factors underlying the beta-band modulation.We found that disappearances of a salient target were associated with beta-band suppression, and target reappearances with beta-band enhancement. This was true for both overt behavioral reports (immediate button presses) and silent counting of the perceptual events. This finding indicates that the beta-band modulation was unrelated to the execution of the motor act associated with a behavioral report of the perceptual decision. Further, changes in pupil-linked arousal, fixational eye movements, or gamma-band responses were not necessary for the beta-band modulation. Together, our results suggest that the beta-band modulation was a top-down signal associated with the process of converting graded perceptual signals into a categorical format underlying flexible behavior. This signal may have been fed back from brain regions involved in decision processing to visual cortex, thus enforcing a “decision-consistent” cortical state.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3835-16.2017

Close

Sorato Minami; Kaoru Amano

Illusory jitter perceived at the frequency of alpha oscillations Journal Article

In: Current Biology, vol. 27, no. 15, pp. 1–13, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Minami2017,
title = {Illusory jitter perceived at the frequency of alpha oscillations},
author = {Sorato Minami and Kaoru Amano},
doi = {10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.033},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Current Biology},
volume = {27},
number = {15},
pages = {1--13},
publisher = {Elsevier Ltd.},
abstract = {Neural oscillations, such as alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and gamma (30–100 Hz), are widespread across cortical areas, and their possible functional roles include feature binding [1], neuronal communication [2, 3], and memory [1, 4]. The most prominent signal among these neural oscillations is the alpha oscillation. Although accumulating evidence suggests that alpha oscillations correlate with various aspects of visual processing [5–18], the number of studies proving their causal contribution in visual perception is limited [11, 16–18]. Here we report that illusory visual vibrations are consciously experienced at the frequency of intrinsic alpha oscillations. We employed an illusory jitter perception termed the motion-induced spatial conflict [19] that originates from the cyclic interaction between motion and shape processing. Comparison between the perceived frequency of illusory jitter and the peak alpha frequency (PAF) measured using magnetoencephalography (MEG) revealed that the inter- and intra-participant variations of the PAF are mirrored by an illusory jitter perception. More crucially, psychophysical and MEG measurements during amplitude-modulated current stimulation [20] showed that the PAF can be artificially manipulated, which results in a corresponding change in the perceived jitter frequency. These results suggest the causal contribution of neural oscillations at the alpha frequency in creating temporal characteristics of visual perception. Our results suggest that cortical areas, dorsal and ventral visual areas in this case, are interacting at the frequency of alpha oscillations [2, 3, 21–27].},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Neural oscillations, such as alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and gamma (30–100 Hz), are widespread across cortical areas, and their possible functional roles include feature binding [1], neuronal communication [2, 3], and memory [1, 4]. The most prominent signal among these neural oscillations is the alpha oscillation. Although accumulating evidence suggests that alpha oscillations correlate with various aspects of visual processing [5–18], the number of studies proving their causal contribution in visual perception is limited [11, 16–18]. Here we report that illusory visual vibrations are consciously experienced at the frequency of intrinsic alpha oscillations. We employed an illusory jitter perception termed the motion-induced spatial conflict [19] that originates from the cyclic interaction between motion and shape processing. Comparison between the perceived frequency of illusory jitter and the peak alpha frequency (PAF) measured using magnetoencephalography (MEG) revealed that the inter- and intra-participant variations of the PAF are mirrored by an illusory jitter perception. More crucially, psychophysical and MEG measurements during amplitude-modulated current stimulation [20] showed that the PAF can be artificially manipulated, which results in a corresponding change in the perceived jitter frequency. These results suggest the causal contribution of neural oscillations at the alpha frequency in creating temporal characteristics of visual perception. Our results suggest that cortical areas, dorsal and ventral visual areas in this case, are interacting at the frequency of alpha oscillations [2, 3, 21–27].

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.033

Close

Tzvetan Popov; Sabine Kastner; Ole Jensen

FEF-controlled alpha delay activity precedes stimulus-induced gamma-band activity in visual cortex Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 37, no. 15, pp. 4117–4127, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Popov2017,
title = {FEF-controlled alpha delay activity precedes stimulus-induced gamma-band activity in visual cortex},
author = {Tzvetan Popov and Sabine Kastner and Ole Jensen},
doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3015-16.2017},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {37},
number = {15},
pages = {4117--4127},
abstract = {Recent findings in the visual system of nonhuman primates have demonstrated an important role of gamma-band activity (40–100 Hz) in the feedforward flow of sensory information, whereas feedback control appears to be established dynamically by oscillations in the alpha (8–13 Hz) and beta (13–18 Hz) bands (van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; Bastos et al., 2015). It is not clear, however, how alpha oscillations are controlled and how they interact with the flow of visual information mediated by gamma-band activity. Using noninvasive human MEG recordings in subjects performing a visuospatial attention task, we show that fluctuations in alpha power during a delay period in a spatial attention task preceded subsequent stimulus-driven gamma-band activity. Importantly, these interactions correlated with behavioral performance. Using Granger analysis, we further show that the right frontal-eye field (rFEF) exerted feedback control of the visual alpha oscillations. Our findings suggest that alpha oscillations controlled by the FEF route cortical information flow by modulating gamma-band activity.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Recent findings in the visual system of nonhuman primates have demonstrated an important role of gamma-band activity (40–100 Hz) in the feedforward flow of sensory information, whereas feedback control appears to be established dynamically by oscillations in the alpha (8–13 Hz) and beta (13–18 Hz) bands (van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; Bastos et al., 2015). It is not clear, however, how alpha oscillations are controlled and how they interact with the flow of visual information mediated by gamma-band activity. Using noninvasive human MEG recordings in subjects performing a visuospatial attention task, we show that fluctuations in alpha power during a delay period in a spatial attention task preceded subsequent stimulus-driven gamma-band activity. Importantly, these interactions correlated with behavioral performance. Using Granger analysis, we further show that the right frontal-eye field (rFEF) exerted feedback control of the visual alpha oscillations. Our findings suggest that alpha oscillations controlled by the FEF route cortical information flow by modulating gamma-band activity.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3015-16.2017

Close

Malcolm Proudfoot; Gustavo Rohenkohl; Andrew Quinn; Giles L. Colclough; Joanne Wuu; Kevin Talbot; Mark W. Woolrich; Michael Benatar; Anna C. Nobre; Martin R. Turner

Altered cortical beta-band oscillations reflect motor system degeneration in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis Journal Article

In: Human Brain Mapping, vol. 38, pp. 237–254, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Proudfoot2017,
title = {Altered cortical beta-band oscillations reflect motor system degeneration in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis},
author = {Malcolm Proudfoot and Gustavo Rohenkohl and Andrew Quinn and Giles L. Colclough and Joanne Wuu and Kevin Talbot and Mark W. Woolrich and Michael Benatar and Anna C. Nobre and Martin R. Turner},
doi = {10.1002/hbm.23357},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Human Brain Mapping},
volume = {38},
pages = {237--254},
abstract = {Continuous rhythmic neuronal oscillations underpin local and regional cortical communication. The impact of the motor system neurodegenerative syndrome amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) on the neuronal oscillations subserving movement might therefore serve as a sensitive marker of disease activity. Movement preparation and execution are consistently associated with modulations to neuronal oscillation beta (15–30 Hz) power. Cortical beta-band oscillations were measured using magnetoencephalography (MEG) during preparation for, execution, and completion of a visually cued, lateralized motor task that included movement inhibition trials. Eleven “classical” ALS patients, 9 with the primary lateral sclerosis (PLS) phenotype, and 12 asymptomatic carriers of ALS-associated gene mutations were compared with age-similar healthy control groups. Augmented beta desynchronization was observed in both contra- and ipsilateral motor cortices of ALS patients during motor preparation. Movement execution coincided with excess beta desynchronization in asymptomatic mutation carriers. Movement completion was followed by a slowed rebound of beta power in all symptomatic patients, further reflected in delayed hemispheric lateralization for beta rebound in the PLS group. This may correspond to the particular involvement of interhemispheric fibers of the corpus callosum previously demonstrated in diffusion tensor imaging studies. We conclude that the ALS spectrum is characterized by intensified cortical beta desynchronization followed by delayed rebound, concordant with a broader concept of cortical hyperexcitability, possibly through loss of inhibitory interneuronal influences. MEG may potentially detect cortical dysfunction prior to the development of overt symptoms, and thus be able to contribute to the assessment of future neuroprotective strategies.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Continuous rhythmic neuronal oscillations underpin local and regional cortical communication. The impact of the motor system neurodegenerative syndrome amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) on the neuronal oscillations subserving movement might therefore serve as a sensitive marker of disease activity. Movement preparation and execution are consistently associated with modulations to neuronal oscillation beta (15–30 Hz) power. Cortical beta-band oscillations were measured using magnetoencephalography (MEG) during preparation for, execution, and completion of a visually cued, lateralized motor task that included movement inhibition trials. Eleven “classical” ALS patients, 9 with the primary lateral sclerosis (PLS) phenotype, and 12 asymptomatic carriers of ALS-associated gene mutations were compared with age-similar healthy control groups. Augmented beta desynchronization was observed in both contra- and ipsilateral motor cortices of ALS patients during motor preparation. Movement execution coincided with excess beta desynchronization in asymptomatic mutation carriers. Movement completion was followed by a slowed rebound of beta power in all symptomatic patients, further reflected in delayed hemispheric lateralization for beta rebound in the PLS group. This may correspond to the particular involvement of interhemispheric fibers of the corpus callosum previously demonstrated in diffusion tensor imaging studies. We conclude that the ALS spectrum is characterized by intensified cortical beta desynchronization followed by delayed rebound, concordant with a broader concept of cortical hyperexcitability, possibly through loss of inhibitory interneuronal influences. MEG may potentially detect cortical dysfunction prior to the development of overt symptoms, and thus be able to contribute to the assessment of future neuroprotective strategies.

Close

  • doi:10.1002/hbm.23357

Close

Craig G. Richter; Mariana Babo-Rebelo; Denis Schwartz; Catherine Tallon-Baudry

Phase-amplitude coupling at the organism level: The amplitude of spontaneous alpha rhythm fluctuations varies with the phase of the infra-slow gastric basal rhythm Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 146, pp. 951–958, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Richter2017,
title = {Phase-amplitude coupling at the organism level: The amplitude of spontaneous alpha rhythm fluctuations varies with the phase of the infra-slow gastric basal rhythm},
author = {Craig G. Richter and Mariana Babo-Rebelo and Denis Schwartz and Catherine Tallon-Baudry},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.043},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {146},
pages = {951--958},
publisher = {Elsevier},
abstract = {A fundamental feature of the temporal organization of neural activity is phase-amplitude coupling between brain rhythms at different frequencies, where the amplitude of a higher frequency varies according to the phase of a lower frequency. Here, we show that this rule extends to brain-organ interactions. We measured both the infra-slow ($sim$0.05 Hz) rhythm intrinsically generated by the stomach – the gastric basal rhythm – using electrogastrography, and spontaneous brain dynamics with magnetoencephalography during resting-state with eyes open. We found significant phase-amplitude coupling between the infra-slow gastric phase and the amplitude of the cortical alpha rhythm (10–11 Hz), with gastric phase accounting for 8% of the variance of alpha rhythm amplitude fluctuations. Gastric-alpha coupling was localized to the right anterior insula, and bilaterally to occipito-parietal regions. Transfer entropy, a measure of directionality of information transfer, indicates that gastric-alpha coupling is due to an ascending influence from the stomach to both the right anterior insula and occipito-parietal regions. Our results show that phase-amplitude coupling so far only observed within the brain extends to brain-viscera interactions. They further reveal that the temporal structure of spontaneous brain activity depends not only on neuron and network properties endogenous to the brain, but also on the slow electrical rhythm generated by the stomach.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

A fundamental feature of the temporal organization of neural activity is phase-amplitude coupling between brain rhythms at different frequencies, where the amplitude of a higher frequency varies according to the phase of a lower frequency. Here, we show that this rule extends to brain-organ interactions. We measured both the infra-slow ($sim$0.05 Hz) rhythm intrinsically generated by the stomach – the gastric basal rhythm – using electrogastrography, and spontaneous brain dynamics with magnetoencephalography during resting-state with eyes open. We found significant phase-amplitude coupling between the infra-slow gastric phase and the amplitude of the cortical alpha rhythm (10–11 Hz), with gastric phase accounting for 8% of the variance of alpha rhythm amplitude fluctuations. Gastric-alpha coupling was localized to the right anterior insula, and bilaterally to occipito-parietal regions. Transfer entropy, a measure of directionality of information transfer, indicates that gastric-alpha coupling is due to an ascending influence from the stomach to both the right anterior insula and occipito-parietal regions. Our results show that phase-amplitude coupling so far only observed within the brain extends to brain-viscera interactions. They further reveal that the temporal structure of spontaneous brain activity depends not only on neuron and network properties endogenous to the brain, but also on the slow electrical rhythm generated by the stomach.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.043

Close

Tobias Staudigl; Elisabeth Hartl; Soheyl Noachtar; Christian F. Doeller; Ole Jensen

Saccades are phase-locked to alpha oscillations in the occipital and medial temporal lobe enhance memory encoding Journal Article

In: PLoS Biology, vol. 15, no. 12, pp. e2003404, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Staudigl2017,
title = {Saccades are phase-locked to alpha oscillations in the occipital and medial temporal lobe enhance memory encoding},
author = {Tobias Staudigl and Elisabeth Hartl and Soheyl Noachtar and Christian F. Doeller and Ole Jensen},
doi = {10.1101/158758},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {PLoS Biology},
volume = {15},
number = {12},
pages = {e2003404},
abstract = {Efficient sampling of visual information requires a coordination of eye movements and ongoing brain oscillations. Using intracranial and MEG recordings, we show that saccades are locked to the phase of visual alpha oscillations, and that this coordination supports mnemonic encoding of visual scenes. Furthermore, parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortex involvement in this coordination reflects effective vision-to-memory mapping, highlighting the importance of neural oscillations for the interaction between visual and memory domains.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Efficient sampling of visual information requires a coordination of eye movements and ongoing brain oscillations. Using intracranial and MEG recordings, we show that saccades are locked to the phase of visual alpha oscillations, and that this coordination supports mnemonic encoding of visual scenes. Furthermore, parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortex involvement in this coordination reflects effective vision-to-memory mapping, highlighting the importance of neural oscillations for the interaction between visual and memory domains.

Close

  • doi:10.1101/158758

Close

Theresa Wildegger; Freek Ede; Mark W. Woolrich; Céline R. Gillebert; Anna C. Nobre

Preparatory $alpha$-band oscillations reflect spatial gating independently of predictions regarding target identity Journal Article

In: Journal of Neurophysiology, vol. 117, no. 3, pp. 1385–1394, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Wildegger2017,
title = {Preparatory $alpha$-band oscillations reflect spatial gating independently of predictions regarding target identity},
author = {Theresa Wildegger and Freek Ede and Mark W. Woolrich and Céline R. Gillebert and Anna C. Nobre},
doi = {10.1152/jn.00856.2016},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neurophysiology},
volume = {117},
number = {3},
pages = {1385--1394},
abstract = {Preparatory modulations of cortical alpha-band oscillations are a reliable index of the voluntary allocation of covert spatial attention. It is currently unclear whether attentional cues containing information about a target's identity (such as its visual orientation), in addition to its location, might additionally shape preparatory alpha modulations. Here, we explore this question by directly comparing spatial and feature-based attention in the same visual detection task while recording brain activity using magneto-encephalography (MEG). At the behavioural level, preparatory feature-based and spatial attention cues both improved performance, and did so independently of each other. Using MEG, we replicated robust alpha lateralisation following spatial cues: in preparation for a visual target, alpha power decreased contralaterally, and increased ipsilaterally to the attended location. Critically, however, preparatory alpha lateralisation was not significantly modulated by predictions regarding target identity, as carried via the behaviourally effective feature-based attention cues. Furthermore, non-lateralised alpha power during the cue-target interval did not differentiate between uninformative cues and cues carrying feature-based predictions either. Based on these results we propose that preparatory alpha modulations play a role in the gating of information between spatially segregated cortical regions, and are therefore particularly well suited for spatial gating of information.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Preparatory modulations of cortical alpha-band oscillations are a reliable index of the voluntary allocation of covert spatial attention. It is currently unclear whether attentional cues containing information about a target's identity (such as its visual orientation), in addition to its location, might additionally shape preparatory alpha modulations. Here, we explore this question by directly comparing spatial and feature-based attention in the same visual detection task while recording brain activity using magneto-encephalography (MEG). At the behavioural level, preparatory feature-based and spatial attention cues both improved performance, and did so independently of each other. Using MEG, we replicated robust alpha lateralisation following spatial cues: in preparation for a visual target, alpha power decreased contralaterally, and increased ipsilaterally to the attended location. Critically, however, preparatory alpha lateralisation was not significantly modulated by predictions regarding target identity, as carried via the behaviourally effective feature-based attention cues. Furthermore, non-lateralised alpha power during the cue-target interval did not differentiate between uninformative cues and cues carrying feature-based predictions either. Based on these results we propose that preparatory alpha modulations play a role in the gating of information between spatially segregated cortical regions, and are therefore particularly well suited for spatial gating of information.

Close

  • doi:10.1152/jn.00856.2016

Close

Claire K. Naughtin; Kristina Horne; Dana Schneider; Dustin Venini; Ashley York; Paul E. Dux

Do implicit and explicit belief processing share neural substrates? Journal Article

In: Human Brain Mapping, vol. 38, no. 9, pp. 4760–4772, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Naughtin2017,
title = {Do implicit and explicit belief processing share neural substrates?},
author = {Claire K. Naughtin and Kristina Horne and Dana Schneider and Dustin Venini and Ashley York and Paul E. Dux},
doi = {10.1002/hbm.23700},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Human Brain Mapping},
volume = {38},
number = {9},
pages = {4760--4772},
abstract = {Humans rely on their ability to infer another person's mental state to understand and predict others' behavior (“theory of mind,” ToM). Multiple lines of research suggest that not only are humans able to consciously process another person's belief state, but also are able to do so implicitly. Here we explored how general implicit belief states are represented in the brain, compared to those substrates involved in explicit ToM processes. Previous work on this topic has yielded conflicting results, and thus, the extent to which the implicit and explicit ToM systems draw on common neural bases is unclear. Participants were presented with “Sally-Anne” type movies in which a protagonist was falsely led to believe a ball was in one location, only for a puppet to later move it to another location in their absence (false-belief condition). In other movies, the protagonist had their back turned the entire time the puppet moved the ball between the two locations, meaning that they had no opportunity to develop any pre-existing beliefs about the scenario (no-belief condition). Using a group of independently localized explicit ToM brain regions, we found greater activity for false-belief trials, relative to no-belief trials, in the right temporoparietal junction, right superior temporal sulcus, precuneus, and left middle prefrontal gyrus. These findings extend upon previous work on the neural bases of implicit ToM by showing substantial overlap between this system and the explicit ToM system, suggesting that both abilities might recruit a common set of mentalizing processes/functional brain regions.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Humans rely on their ability to infer another person's mental state to understand and predict others' behavior (“theory of mind,” ToM). Multiple lines of research suggest that not only are humans able to consciously process another person's belief state, but also are able to do so implicitly. Here we explored how general implicit belief states are represented in the brain, compared to those substrates involved in explicit ToM processes. Previous work on this topic has yielded conflicting results, and thus, the extent to which the implicit and explicit ToM systems draw on common neural bases is unclear. Participants were presented with “Sally-Anne” type movies in which a protagonist was falsely led to believe a ball was in one location, only for a puppet to later move it to another location in their absence (false-belief condition). In other movies, the protagonist had their back turned the entire time the puppet moved the ball between the two locations, meaning that they had no opportunity to develop any pre-existing beliefs about the scenario (no-belief condition). Using a group of independently localized explicit ToM brain regions, we found greater activity for false-belief trials, relative to no-belief trials, in the right temporoparietal junction, right superior temporal sulcus, precuneus, and left middle prefrontal gyrus. These findings extend upon previous work on the neural bases of implicit ToM by showing substantial overlap between this system and the explicit ToM system, suggesting that both abilities might recruit a common set of mentalizing processes/functional brain regions.

Close

  • doi:10.1002/hbm.23700

Close

Veerle Neyens; Rose Bruffaerts; Antonietta G. Liuzzi; Ioannis Kalfas; Ronald Peeters; Emmanuel Keuleers; Rufin Vogels; Simon De Deyne; Gert Storms; Patrick Dupont; Rik Vandenberghe

Representation of semantic similarity in the left intraparietal sulcus: Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence Journal Article

In: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 11, pp. 402, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Neyens2017,
title = {Representation of semantic similarity in the left intraparietal sulcus: Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence},
author = {Veerle Neyens and Rose Bruffaerts and Antonietta G. Liuzzi and Ioannis Kalfas and Ronald Peeters and Emmanuel Keuleers and Rufin Vogels and Simon De Deyne and Gert Storms and Patrick Dupont and Rik Vandenberghe},
doi = {10.3389/fnhum.2017.00402},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Frontiers in Human Neuroscience},
volume = {11},
pages = {402},
abstract = {According to a recent study, semantic similarity between concrete entities correlates with the similarity of activity patterns in left middle IPS during category naming. We examined the replicability of this effect under passive viewing conditions, the potential role of visuoperceptual similarity, where the effect is situated compared to regions that have been previously implicated in visuospatial attention, and how it compares to effects of object identity and location. Forty-six subjects participated. Subjects passively viewed pictures from two categories, musical instruments and vehicles. Semantic similarity between entities was estimated based on a concept-feature matrix obtained in more than 1,000 subjects. Visuoperceptual similarity was modeled based on the HMAX model, the AlexNet deep convolutional learning model, and thirdly, based on subjective visuoperceptual similarity ratings. Among the IPS regions examined, only left middle IPS showed a semantic similarity effect. The effect was significant in hIP1, hIP2, and hIP3. Visuoperceptual similarity did not correlate with similarity of activity patterns in left middle IPS. The semantic similarity effect in left middle IPS was significantly stronger than in the right middle IPS and also stronger than in the left or right posterior IPS. The semantic similarity effect was similar to that seen in the angular gyrus. Object identity effects were much more widespread across nearly all parietal areas examined. Location effects were relatively specific for posterior IPS and area 7 bilaterally. To conclude, the current findings replicate the semantic similarity effect in left middle IPS under passive viewing conditions, and demonstrate its anatomical specificity within a cytoarchitectonic reference frame. We propose that the semantic similarity effect in left middle IPS reflects the transient uploading of semantic representations in working memory.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

According to a recent study, semantic similarity between concrete entities correlates with the similarity of activity patterns in left middle IPS during category naming. We examined the replicability of this effect under passive viewing conditions, the potential role of visuoperceptual similarity, where the effect is situated compared to regions that have been previously implicated in visuospatial attention, and how it compares to effects of object identity and location. Forty-six subjects participated. Subjects passively viewed pictures from two categories, musical instruments and vehicles. Semantic similarity between entities was estimated based on a concept-feature matrix obtained in more than 1,000 subjects. Visuoperceptual similarity was modeled based on the HMAX model, the AlexNet deep convolutional learning model, and thirdly, based on subjective visuoperceptual similarity ratings. Among the IPS regions examined, only left middle IPS showed a semantic similarity effect. The effect was significant in hIP1, hIP2, and hIP3. Visuoperceptual similarity did not correlate with similarity of activity patterns in left middle IPS. The semantic similarity effect in left middle IPS was significantly stronger than in the right middle IPS and also stronger than in the left or right posterior IPS. The semantic similarity effect was similar to that seen in the angular gyrus. Object identity effects were much more widespread across nearly all parietal areas examined. Location effects were relatively specific for posterior IPS and area 7 bilaterally. To conclude, the current findings replicate the semantic similarity effect in left middle IPS under passive viewing conditions, and demonstrate its anatomical specificity within a cytoarchitectonic reference frame. We propose that the semantic similarity effect in left middle IPS reflects the transient uploading of semantic representations in working memory.

Close

  • doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00402

Close

Abigail L. Noyce; Nishmar Cestero; Samantha W. Michalka; Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham; David C. Somers

Sensory-biased and multiple-demand processing in human lateral frontal cortex Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 37, no. 36, pp. 8755– 8766, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Noyce2017,
title = {Sensory-biased and multiple-demand processing in human lateral frontal cortex},
author = {Abigail L. Noyce and Nishmar Cestero and Samantha W. Michalka and Barbara G. Shinn-Cunningham and David C. Somers},
doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0660-17.2017},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {37},
number = {36},
pages = {8755-- 8766},
abstract = {The functionality of much of human lateral frontal cortex (LFC) has been characterized as 'multiple demand' as these regions appear to support a broad range of cognitive tasks. In contrast to this domain-general account, recent evidence indicates that portions of LFC are consistently selective for sensory modality. Michalka et al. (2015) reported two bilateral regions that are biased for visual attention, superior precentral sulcus (sPCS) and inferior precentral sulcus (iPCS), interleaved with two bilateral regions that are biased for auditory attention, transverse gyrus intersecting precentral sulcus (tgPCS) and caudal inferior frontal sulcus (cIFS). In the present study, we employ functional MRI to examine both the multiple-demand and sensory-bias hypotheses within caudal portions of human LFC (both men and women participated). Using visual and auditory 2-back tasks, we replicate the finding of two bilateral visual-biased and two bilateral auditory-biased LFC regions, corresponding to sPCS & iPCS and to tgPCS & cIFS, and demonstrate high within-subject reliability of these regions over time and across tasks. In addition, we assess multiple demand responsiveness using BOLD signal recruitment and vector space analysis. In both, we find that the two visual-biased regions, sPCS & iPCS, exhibit stronger multiple demand responsiveness than do the auditory-biased LFC regions, tgPCS & cIFS; however, neither reaches the degree of multiple demand responsiveness exhibited by dorsal anterior cingulate/pre-supplemental motor area or by anterior insula. These results reconcile two competing views of LFC by demonstrating the coexistence of sensory specialization and multiple demand functionality, especially in visual-biased LFC structures.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

The functionality of much of human lateral frontal cortex (LFC) has been characterized as 'multiple demand' as these regions appear to support a broad range of cognitive tasks. In contrast to this domain-general account, recent evidence indicates that portions of LFC are consistently selective for sensory modality. Michalka et al. (2015) reported two bilateral regions that are biased for visual attention, superior precentral sulcus (sPCS) and inferior precentral sulcus (iPCS), interleaved with two bilateral regions that are biased for auditory attention, transverse gyrus intersecting precentral sulcus (tgPCS) and caudal inferior frontal sulcus (cIFS). In the present study, we employ functional MRI to examine both the multiple-demand and sensory-bias hypotheses within caudal portions of human LFC (both men and women participated). Using visual and auditory 2-back tasks, we replicate the finding of two bilateral visual-biased and two bilateral auditory-biased LFC regions, corresponding to sPCS & iPCS and to tgPCS & cIFS, and demonstrate high within-subject reliability of these regions over time and across tasks. In addition, we assess multiple demand responsiveness using BOLD signal recruitment and vector space analysis. In both, we find that the two visual-biased regions, sPCS & iPCS, exhibit stronger multiple demand responsiveness than do the auditory-biased LFC regions, tgPCS & cIFS; however, neither reaches the degree of multiple demand responsiveness exhibited by dorsal anterior cingulate/pre-supplemental motor area or by anterior insula. These results reconcile two competing views of LFC by demonstrating the coexistence of sensory specialization and multiple demand functionality, especially in visual-biased LFC structures.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0660-17.2017

Close

Lauri Nummenmaa; Lauri Oksama; Enrico Glerean; Jukka Hyönä

Cortical circuit for binding object identity and location during multiple-object tracking Journal Article

In: Cerebral Cortex, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 162–172, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Nummenmaa2017,
title = {Cortical circuit for binding object identity and location during multiple-object tracking},
author = {Lauri Nummenmaa and Lauri Oksama and Enrico Glerean and Jukka Hyönä},
doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhw380},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
volume = {27},
number = {1},
pages = {162--172},
abstract = {Sustained multifocal attention for moving targets requires binding object identities with their locations. The brain mechanisms of identity-location binding during attentive tracking have remained unresolved. In 2 functional magnetic resonance imaging experiments, we measured participants' hemodynamic activity during attentive tracking of multiple objects with equivalent (multiple-object tracking) versus distinct (multiple identity tracking, MIT) identities. Task load was manipulated parametrically. Both tasks activated large frontoparietal circuits. MIT led to significantly increased activity in frontoparietal and temporal systems subserving object recognition and working memory. These effects were replicated when eye movements were prohibited. MIT was associated with significantly increased functional connectivity between lateral temporal and frontal and parietal regions. We propose that coordinated activity of this network subserves identity-location binding during attentive tracking.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Sustained multifocal attention for moving targets requires binding object identities with their locations. The brain mechanisms of identity-location binding during attentive tracking have remained unresolved. In 2 functional magnetic resonance imaging experiments, we measured participants' hemodynamic activity during attentive tracking of multiple objects with equivalent (multiple-object tracking) versus distinct (multiple identity tracking, MIT) identities. Task load was manipulated parametrically. Both tasks activated large frontoparietal circuits. MIT led to significantly increased activity in frontoparietal and temporal systems subserving object recognition and working memory. These effects were replicated when eye movements were prohibited. MIT was associated with significantly increased functional connectivity between lateral temporal and frontal and parietal regions. We propose that coordinated activity of this network subserves identity-location binding during attentive tracking.

Close

  • doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw380

Close

E. Oberwelland; Leonhard Schilbach; I. Barisic; Sarah C. Krall; K. Vogeley; Gereon R. Fink; B. Herpertz-Dahlmann; Kerstin Konrad; Martin Schulte-Rüther

Young adolescents with autism show abnormal joint attention network: A gaze contingent fMRI study Journal Article

In: NeuroImage: Clinical, vol. 14, pp. 112–121, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Oberwelland2017,
title = {Young adolescents with autism show abnormal joint attention network: A gaze contingent fMRI study},
author = {E. Oberwelland and Leonhard Schilbach and I. Barisic and Sarah C. Krall and K. Vogeley and Gereon R. Fink and B. Herpertz-Dahlmann and Kerstin Konrad and Martin Schulte-Rüther},
doi = {10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.006},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage: Clinical},
volume = {14},
pages = {112--121},
publisher = {The Authors},
abstract = {Behavioral research has revealed deficits in the development of joint attention (JA) as one of the earliest signs of autism. While the neural basis of JA has been studied predominantly in adults, we recently demonstrated a protracted development of the brain networks supporting JA in typically developing children and adolescents. The present eye-tracking/fMRI study now extends these findings to adolescents with autism. Our results show that in adolescents with autism JA is subserved by abnormal activation patterns in brain areas related to social cognition abnormalities which are at the core of ASD including the STS and TPJ, despite behavioral maturation with no behavioral differences. Furthermore, in the autism group we observed increased neural activity in a network of social and emotional processing areas during interactions with their mother. Moreover, data indicated that less severely affected individuals with autism showed higher frontal activation associated with self-initiated interactions. Taken together, this study provides first-time data of JA in children/adolescents with autism incorporating the interactive character of JA, its reciprocity and motivational aspects. The observed functional differences in adolescents ASD suggest that persistent developmental differences in the neural processes underlying JA contribute to social interaction difficulties in ASD.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Behavioral research has revealed deficits in the development of joint attention (JA) as one of the earliest signs of autism. While the neural basis of JA has been studied predominantly in adults, we recently demonstrated a protracted development of the brain networks supporting JA in typically developing children and adolescents. The present eye-tracking/fMRI study now extends these findings to adolescents with autism. Our results show that in adolescents with autism JA is subserved by abnormal activation patterns in brain areas related to social cognition abnormalities which are at the core of ASD including the STS and TPJ, despite behavioral maturation with no behavioral differences. Furthermore, in the autism group we observed increased neural activity in a network of social and emotional processing areas during interactions with their mother. Moreover, data indicated that less severely affected individuals with autism showed higher frontal activation associated with self-initiated interactions. Taken together, this study provides first-time data of JA in children/adolescents with autism incorporating the interactive character of JA, its reciprocity and motivational aspects. The observed functional differences in adolescents ASD suggest that persistent developmental differences in the neural processes underlying JA contribute to social interaction difficulties in ASD.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.006

Close

Joel Reithler; Judith C. Peters; Rainer Goebel

Characterizing object- and position-dependent response profiles to uni- and bilateral stimulus configurations in human higher visual cortex: A 7T fMRI study Journal Article

In: NeuroImage, vol. 152, pp. 551–562, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Reithler2017,
title = {Characterizing object- and position-dependent response profiles to uni- and bilateral stimulus configurations in human higher visual cortex: A 7T fMRI study},
author = {Joel Reithler and Judith C. Peters and Rainer Goebel},
doi = {10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.038},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {NeuroImage},
volume = {152},
pages = {551--562},
abstract = {Visual scenes are initially processed via segregated neural pathways dedicated to either of the two visual hemifields. Although higher-order visual areas are generally believed to utilize invariant object representations (abstracted away from features such as stimulus position), recent findings suggest they retain more spatial information than previously thought. Here, we assessed the nature of such higher-order object representations in human cortex using high-resolution fMRI at 7T, supported by corroborative 3T data. We show that multi-voxel activation patterns in both the contra- and ipsilateral hemisphere can be exploited to successfully classify the object category of unilaterally presented stimuli. Moreover, robustly identified rank order-based response profiles demonstrated a strong contralateral bias which frequently outweighed object category preferences. Finally, we contrasted different combinatorial operations to predict the responses during bilateral stimulation conditions based on responses to their constituent unilateral elements. Results favored a max operation predominantly reflecting the contralateral stimuli. The current findings extend previous work by showing that configuration-dependent modulations in higher-order visual cortex responses as observed in single unit activity have a counterpart in human neural population coding. They furthermore corroborate the emerging view that position coding is a fundamental functional characteristic of ventral visual stream processing.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Visual scenes are initially processed via segregated neural pathways dedicated to either of the two visual hemifields. Although higher-order visual areas are generally believed to utilize invariant object representations (abstracted away from features such as stimulus position), recent findings suggest they retain more spatial information than previously thought. Here, we assessed the nature of such higher-order object representations in human cortex using high-resolution fMRI at 7T, supported by corroborative 3T data. We show that multi-voxel activation patterns in both the contra- and ipsilateral hemisphere can be exploited to successfully classify the object category of unilaterally presented stimuli. Moreover, robustly identified rank order-based response profiles demonstrated a strong contralateral bias which frequently outweighed object category preferences. Finally, we contrasted different combinatorial operations to predict the responses during bilateral stimulation conditions based on responses to their constituent unilateral elements. Results favored a max operation predominantly reflecting the contralateral stimuli. The current findings extend previous work by showing that configuration-dependent modulations in higher-order visual cortex responses as observed in single unit activity have a counterpart in human neural population coding. They furthermore corroborate the emerging view that position coding is a fundamental functional characteristic of ventral visual stream processing.

Close

  • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.038

Close

Christiane S. Rohr; Sarah A. Vinette; Kari A. L. Parsons; Ivy Y. K. Cho; Dennis Dimond; Alina Benischek; Catherine Lebel; Deborah Dewey; Signe Bray

Functional connectivity of the dorsal attention network predicts selective attention in 4–7 year-old girls Journal Article

In: Cerebral Cortex, vol. 27, no. 9, pp. 4350–4360, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Rohr2017,
title = {Functional connectivity of the dorsal attention network predicts selective attention in 4–7 year-old girls},
author = {Christiane S. Rohr and Sarah A. Vinette and Kari A. L. Parsons and Ivy Y. K. Cho and Dennis Dimond and Alina Benischek and Catherine Lebel and Deborah Dewey and Signe Bray},
doi = {10.1093/cercor/bhw236},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Cerebral Cortex},
volume = {27},
number = {9},
pages = {4350--4360},
abstract = {Early childhood is a period of profound neural development and remodeling during which attention skills undergo rapid maturation. Attention networks have been extensively studied in the adult brain, yet relatively little is known about changes in early childhood, and their relation to cognitive development. We investigated the association between age and functional connectivity (FC) within the dorsal attention network (DAN) and the association between FC and attention skills in early childhood. Functional magnetic resonance imaging data was collected during passive viewing in 44 typically developing female children between 4 and 7 years whose sustained, selective, and executive attention skills were assessed. FC of the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and the frontal eye fields (FEF) was computed across the entire brain and regressed against age. Age was positively associated with FC between core nodes of the DAN, the IPS and the FEF, and negatively associated with FC between the DAN and regions of the default-mode network. Further, controlling for age, FC between the IPS and FEF was significantly associated with selective attention. These findings add to our understanding of early childhood development of attention networks and suggest that greater FC within the DAN is associated with better selective attention skills.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Early childhood is a period of profound neural development and remodeling during which attention skills undergo rapid maturation. Attention networks have been extensively studied in the adult brain, yet relatively little is known about changes in early childhood, and their relation to cognitive development. We investigated the association between age and functional connectivity (FC) within the dorsal attention network (DAN) and the association between FC and attention skills in early childhood. Functional magnetic resonance imaging data was collected during passive viewing in 44 typically developing female children between 4 and 7 years whose sustained, selective, and executive attention skills were assessed. FC of the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and the frontal eye fields (FEF) was computed across the entire brain and regressed against age. Age was positively associated with FC between core nodes of the DAN, the IPS and the FEF, and negatively associated with FC between the DAN and regions of the default-mode network. Further, controlling for age, FC between the IPS and FEF was significantly associated with selective attention. These findings add to our understanding of early childhood development of attention networks and suggest that greater FC within the DAN is associated with better selective attention skills.

Close

  • doi:10.1093/cercor/bhw236

Close

Annie L. Shelton; Kim M. Cornish; Meaghan Clough; Sanuji Gajamange; Scott Kolbe; Joanne Fielding

Disassociation between brain activation and executive function in fragile X premutation females Journal Article

In: Human Brain Mapping, vol. 38, no. 2, pp. 1056–1067, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{Shelton2017,
title = {Disassociation between brain activation and executive function in fragile X premutation females},
author = {Annie L. Shelton and Kim M. Cornish and Meaghan Clough and Sanuji Gajamange and Scott Kolbe and Joanne Fielding},
doi = {10.1002/hbm.23438},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Human Brain Mapping},
volume = {38},
number = {2},
pages = {1056--1067},
abstract = {Executive dysfunction has been demonstrated among premutation (PM) carriers (55-199 CGG repeats) of the Fragile X mental retardation 1 (FMR1) gene. Further, alterations to neural activation patterns have been reported during memory and comparison based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) tasks in these carriers. For the first time, the relationships between fMRI neural activation during an interleaved ocular motor prosaccade/antisaccade paradigm, and concurrent task performance (saccade measures of latency, accuracy and error rate) in PM females were examined. Although no differences were found in whole brain activation patterns, regions of interest (ROI) analyses revealed reduced activation in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) during antisaccade trials for PM females. Further, a series of divergent and group specific relationships were found between ROI activation and saccade measures. Specifically, for control females, activation within the right VLPFC and supramarginal gyrus correlated negatively with antisaccade latencies, while for PM females, activation within these regions was found to negatively correlate with antisaccade accuracy and error rate (right VLPFC only). For control females, activation within frontal and supplementary eye fields and bilateral intraparietal sulci correlated with prosaccade latency and accuracy; however, no significant prosaccade correlations were found for PM females. This exploratory study extends previous reports of altered prefrontal neural engagement in PM carriers, and clearly demonstrates dissociation between control and PM females in the transformation of neural activation into overt measures of executive dysfunction.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Executive dysfunction has been demonstrated among premutation (PM) carriers (55-199 CGG repeats) of the Fragile X mental retardation 1 (FMR1) gene. Further, alterations to neural activation patterns have been reported during memory and comparison based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) tasks in these carriers. For the first time, the relationships between fMRI neural activation during an interleaved ocular motor prosaccade/antisaccade paradigm, and concurrent task performance (saccade measures of latency, accuracy and error rate) in PM females were examined. Although no differences were found in whole brain activation patterns, regions of interest (ROI) analyses revealed reduced activation in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) during antisaccade trials for PM females. Further, a series of divergent and group specific relationships were found between ROI activation and saccade measures. Specifically, for control females, activation within the right VLPFC and supramarginal gyrus correlated negatively with antisaccade latencies, while for PM females, activation within these regions was found to negatively correlate with antisaccade accuracy and error rate (right VLPFC only). For control females, activation within frontal and supplementary eye fields and bilateral intraparietal sulci correlated with prosaccade latency and accuracy; however, no significant prosaccade correlations were found for PM females. This exploratory study extends previous reports of altered prefrontal neural engagement in PM carriers, and clearly demonstrates dissociation between control and PM females in the transformation of neural activation into overt measures of executive dysfunction.

Close

  • doi:10.1002/hbm.23438

Close

Maryam Vaziri-Pashkam; Yaoda Xu

Goal-directed visual processing differentially impacts human ventral and dorsal visual representations Journal Article

In: Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 37, no. 36, pp. 8767–8782, 2017.

Abstract | Links | BibTeX

@article{VaziriPashkam2017,
title = {Goal-directed visual processing differentially impacts human ventral and dorsal visual representations},
author = {Maryam Vaziri-Pashkam and Yaoda Xu},
doi = {10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3392-16.2017},
year = {2017},
date = {2017-01-01},
journal = {Journal of Neuroscience},
volume = {37},
number = {36},
pages = {8767--8782},
abstract = {Recent studies have challenged the ventral/“what” and dorsal/“where” two-visual-processing-pathway view by showing the existence of “what”and“where”information in both pathways. Is thetwo-pathwaydistinction still valid? Here,weexaminedhowgoal-directed visual information processing may differentially impact visual representations in these two pathways. Using fMRI and multivariate pattern analysis, in three experiments onhumanparticipants (57% females), by manipulating whether color or shape was task-relevant andhow they were conjoined, we examined shape-based object category decoding in occipitotemporal and parietal regions.Wefound that object category representations in all the regions examined were influenced by whether or not object shape was task-relevant. This task effect, however,tendedto decrease as task-relevantandirrelevant featuresweremoreintegrated, reflecting thewell-knownobject-based feature encoding. Interestingly, task relevance played a relatively minor role in driving the representational structures of early visual and ventral object regions. They were driven predominantly by variations in object shapes. In contrast, the effect of task was much greater in dorsal than ventral regions, with object category and task relevance both contributing significantly to the representational structures of the dorsal regions. These results showed that, whereas visual representations in the ventral pathway are more invariant and reflect “what an object is,” those in the dorsal pathway are more adaptive and reflect “what we do with it.” Thus, despite the existence of “what” and “where” information in both visual processing pathways, the two pathways may still differ fundamentally in their roles in visual infor- mation representation.},
keywords = {},
pubstate = {published},
tppubtype = {article}
}

Close

Recent studies have challenged the ventral/“what” and dorsal/“where” two-visual-processing-pathway view by showing the existence of “what”and“where”information in both pathways. Is thetwo-pathwaydistinction still valid? Here,weexaminedhowgoal-directed visual information processing may differentially impact visual representations in these two pathways. Using fMRI and multivariate pattern analysis, in three experiments onhumanparticipants (57% females), by manipulating whether color or shape was task-relevant andhow they were conjoined, we examined shape-based object category decoding in occipitotemporal and parietal regions.Wefound that object category representations in all the regions examined were influenced by whether or not object shape was task-relevant. This task effect, however,tendedto decrease as task-relevantandirrelevant featuresweremoreintegrated, reflecting thewell-knownobject-based feature encoding. Interestingly, task relevance played a relatively minor role in driving the representational structures of early visual and ventral object regions. They were driven predominantly by variations in object shapes. In contrast, the effect of task was much greater in dorsal than ventral regions, with object category and task relevance both contributing significantly to the representational structures of the dorsal regions. These results showed that, whereas visual representations in the ventral pathway are more invariant and reflect “what an object is,” those in the dorsal pathway are more adaptive and reflect “what we do with it.” Thus, despite the existence of “what” and “where” information in both visual processing pathways, the two pathways may still differ fundamentally in their roles in visual infor- mation representation.

Close

  • doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3392-16.2017

Close

575 entries « ‹ 4 of 6 › »

Let’s Keep in Touch

  • Twitter
  • Facebook
  • Instagram
  • LinkedIn
  • YouTube
Newsletter
Newsletter Archive
Conferences

Contact

info@sr-research.com

Phone: +1-613-271-8686

Toll Free: +1-866-821-0731

Fax: +1-613-482-4866

Quick Links

Products

Solutions

Support Forum

Legal

Legal Notice

Privacy Policy | Accessibility Policy

EyeLink® eye trackers are intended for research purposes only and should not be used in the treatment or diagnosis of any medical condition.

Featured Blog

Reading Profiles of Adults with Dyslexia

Reading Profile of Adults with Dyslexia


Copyright © 2023 · SR Research Ltd. All Rights Reserved. EyeLink is a registered trademark of SR Research Ltd.